删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

达者何以兼济天下:高阶层再分配偏向的心理机制及谦卑的作用

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

白洁1, 杨沈龙2, 徐步霄3, 郭永玉1()
1南京师范大学心理学院, 南京 210097
2西安交通大学人文社会科学学院社会心理学研究所, 西安 710049
3绍兴文理学院大脑、心智与教育研究中心, 浙江 绍兴 312000
收稿日期:2020-08-19出版日期:2021-10-25发布日期:2021-08-23
通讯作者:郭永玉E-mail:yyguo@njnu.edu.cn

基金资助:国家自然科学基金面上项目(71971120);国家社会科学基金重点项目(20AZD084)

How can successful people share their goodness with the world: The psychological mechanism underlying the upper social classes’ redistributive preferences and the role of humility

BAI Jie1, YANG Shenlong2, XU Buxiao3, GUO Yongyu1()
1School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing 210097, China
2Institute of Social Psychology, School of Humanities and Social Science, Xi’an Jiaotong University, Xi’an 710049, China
3Center for Brain, Mind and Education, Shaoxing University, Zhejiang, Shaoxing 312000, China
Received:2020-08-19Online:2021-10-25Published:2021-08-23
Contact:GUO Yongyu E-mail:yyguo@njnu.edu.cn






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 一些欧美研究发现高阶层者与低阶层者相比再分配偏向更低, 但是这一结论尚缺少跨文化一致性的证据, 而且对于这一现象的心理机制和干预策略的探讨也相对不足。基于此, 本研究通过3个子研究分别来关注我国民众再分配偏向的阶层差异及其心理机制, 并探讨可能的干预策略。结果发现:(1)与西方社会的情形相似, 在中国社会高阶层者的再分配偏向也显著低于低阶层者; (2)社会阶层对再分配偏向的影响, 部分是通过贫富差距归因倾向起作用的:与低阶层者相比, 高阶层者更倾向于将贫富差距归因于能力、努力、志向或抱负等个体内部因素, 进而再分配偏向更低; (3)通过启动高阶层者谦卑的心态, 能够降低其贫富差距内归因倾向, 进而提升其再分配偏向。此结果验证了社会阶层不平等维持模型的重要观点, 同时也为推动社会再分配、促进发展成果共享等提供了一定的社会治理启示。


表1各变量的描述性统计和相关分析
变量 %/M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1性别 51.50
2年龄 51.02 16.17 0.02
3民族 92.60 0.00 -0.03**
4宗教 11.40 -0.08*** -0.04*** -0.22***
5政治 90.20 0.02 0.02 0.00 0.00
6婚姻 20.60 -0.03** 0.03** 0.01 0.02 -0.02
7客观 0.00 0.86 -0.15*** -0.40*** -0.10*** 0.07*** -0.03** 0.00
8主观 4.35 1.61 0.03* 0.00 -0.04*** 0.00 0.02 0.06*** 0.25***
9 家庭 2.67 0.72 -0.01 -0.07*** -0.05*** 0.00 0.00 0.07*** 0.31** 0.45***
10个人 1.73 0.59 -0.02* 0.00 -0.05*** 0.01 0.00 0.05*** 0.25*** 0.40*** 0.55***
11偏向 3.78 0.93 0.02 0.10*** 0.03* -0.03* 0.02 0.00 -0.12*** -0.06*** -0.08*** -0.06***

表1各变量的描述性统计和相关分析
变量 %/M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1性别 51.50
2年龄 51.02 16.17 0.02
3民族 92.60 0.00 -0.03**
4宗教 11.40 -0.08*** -0.04*** -0.22***
5政治 90.20 0.02 0.02 0.00 0.00
6婚姻 20.60 -0.03** 0.03** 0.01 0.02 -0.02
7客观 0.00 0.86 -0.15*** -0.40*** -0.10*** 0.07*** -0.03** 0.00
8主观 4.35 1.61 0.03* 0.00 -0.04*** 0.00 0.02 0.06*** 0.25***
9 家庭 2.67 0.72 -0.01 -0.07*** -0.05*** 0.00 0.00 0.07*** 0.31** 0.45***
10个人 1.73 0.59 -0.02* 0.00 -0.05*** 0.01 0.00 0.05*** 0.25*** 0.40*** 0.55***
11偏向 3.78 0.93 0.02 0.10*** 0.03* -0.03* 0.02 0.00 -0.12*** -0.06*** -0.08*** -0.06***


表2各变量的描述性统计和相关分析
变量 %/M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1性别 41.70
2 年龄 32.73 8.73 -0.04
3 民族 94.20 0.00 -0.05
4 户口 60.20 -0.05 -0.11** -0.05
5 政治 19.00 0.13** -0.08* -0.04 0.20***
6 宗教 90.30 -0.07 -0.07 0.32*** 0.02 0.12**
7 婚姻 61.80 0.13*** 0.49*** -0.08* -0.10* -0.04 -0.14***
8 客观 0.01 0.76 -0.14*** -0.18*** 0.10* -0.30*** -0.27*** 0.12** -0.26***
9 主观 4.15 1.88 0.04 -0.07 -0.02 -0.13** -0.04 -0.03 -0.02 0.30***
10归因 0.27 1.05 0.02 0.00 -0.01 0.10* 0.11** -0.05 0.00 -0.07 0.22***
11偏向 5.35 1.00 0.03 0.23*** 0.05 0.02 0.03 0.14*** 0.09* 0.01 -0.21*** -0.15***

表2各变量的描述性统计和相关分析
变量 %/M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1性别 41.70
2 年龄 32.73 8.73 -0.04
3 民族 94.20 0.00 -0.05
4 户口 60.20 -0.05 -0.11** -0.05
5 政治 19.00 0.13** -0.08* -0.04 0.20***
6 宗教 90.30 -0.07 -0.07 0.32*** 0.02 0.12**
7 婚姻 61.80 0.13*** 0.49*** -0.08* -0.10* -0.04 -0.14***
8 客观 0.01 0.76 -0.14*** -0.18*** 0.10* -0.30*** -0.27*** 0.12** -0.26***
9 主观 4.15 1.88 0.04 -0.07 -0.02 -0.13** -0.04 -0.03 -0.02 0.30***
10归因 0.27 1.05 0.02 0.00 -0.01 0.10* 0.11** -0.05 0.00 -0.07 0.22***
11偏向 5.35 1.00 0.03 0.23*** 0.05 0.02 0.03 0.14*** 0.09* 0.01 -0.21*** -0.15***


表3内归因倾向在主观阶层预测效应中的中介作用分析
因变量 R2 β SE t 95% CI
内归因倾向作
因变量
0.09
年龄 0.03 0.05 0.64 [-0.06, 0.12]
户口类型 0.09 0.04 2.32* [0.01, 0.17]
政治面貌 0.16 0.04 3.76*** [0.08, 0.25]
宗教信仰 -0.06 0.04 -1.44 [-0.13, 0.02]
婚姻状况 -0.07 0.05 -1.46 [-0.17, 0.02]
主观阶层 0.24 0.04 6.11*** [0.16, 0.32]
再分配偏向作
因变量
0.14
年龄 0.25 0.04 5.77*** [0.17, 0.34]
户口类型 0.03 0.04 0.78 [-0.05, 0.11]
政治面貌 0.01 0.04 0.19 [-0.08, 0.09]
宗教信仰 0.15 0.04 3.82*** [0.07, 0.22]
婚姻状况 -0.02 0.05 -0.38 [-0.11, 0.08]
主观阶层 -0.17 0.04 -4.20*** [-0.24, -0.09]
内归因倾向 -0.11 0.04 -2.85* [-0.19, -0.04]

表3内归因倾向在主观阶层预测效应中的中介作用分析
因变量 R2 β SE t 95% CI
内归因倾向作
因变量
0.09
年龄 0.03 0.05 0.64 [-0.06, 0.12]
户口类型 0.09 0.04 2.32* [0.01, 0.17]
政治面貌 0.16 0.04 3.76*** [0.08, 0.25]
宗教信仰 -0.06 0.04 -1.44 [-0.13, 0.02]
婚姻状况 -0.07 0.05 -1.46 [-0.17, 0.02]
主观阶层 0.24 0.04 6.11*** [0.16, 0.32]
再分配偏向作
因变量
0.14
年龄 0.25 0.04 5.77*** [0.17, 0.34]
户口类型 0.03 0.04 0.78 [-0.05, 0.11]
政治面貌 0.01 0.04 0.19 [-0.08, 0.09]
宗教信仰 0.15 0.04 3.82*** [0.07, 0.22]
婚姻状况 -0.02 0.05 -0.38 [-0.11, 0.08]
主观阶层 -0.17 0.04 -4.20*** [-0.24, -0.09]
内归因倾向 -0.11 0.04 -2.85* [-0.19, -0.04]


表4内归因倾向在客观阶层预测效应中的中介作用分析
因变量 R2 β SE t 95% CI
内归因倾向作因变量 0.03
年龄 0.03 0.05 0.64 [-0.06, 0.12]
户口类型 0.04 0.04 0.96 [-0.04, 0.13]
政治面貌 0.18 0.05 3.82*** [0.09, 0.28]
宗教信仰 -0.04 0.04 -0.99 [-0.13,.04]
婚姻状况 -0.08 0.05 -1.54 [-.18, 0.02]
客观阶层 0.03 0.06 0.42 [-0.10, 0.15]
再分配偏向作因变量 0.10
年龄 0.27 0.05 5.86*** [0.18, 0.36]
户口类型 0.06 0.04 1.39 [-0.02, 0.14]
政治面貌 0.04 0.05 0.90 [-0.05, 0.14]
宗教信仰 0.14 0.04 3.34*** [0.06, 0.23]
婚姻状况 -0.02 0.05 -0.35 [-0.12, 0.08]
客观阶层 0.08 0.06 1.31 [-0.04, 0.21]
内归因倾向 -0.15 0.04 -3.65*** [-0.23, -0.07]

表4内归因倾向在客观阶层预测效应中的中介作用分析
因变量 R2 β SE t 95% CI
内归因倾向作因变量 0.03
年龄 0.03 0.05 0.64 [-0.06, 0.12]
户口类型 0.04 0.04 0.96 [-0.04, 0.13]
政治面貌 0.18 0.05 3.82*** [0.09, 0.28]
宗教信仰 -0.04 0.04 -0.99 [-0.13,.04]
婚姻状况 -0.08 0.05 -1.54 [-.18, 0.02]
客观阶层 0.03 0.06 0.42 [-0.10, 0.15]
再分配偏向作因变量 0.10
年龄 0.27 0.05 5.86*** [0.18, 0.36]
户口类型 0.06 0.04 1.39 [-0.02, 0.14]
政治面貌 0.04 0.05 0.90 [-0.05, 0.14]
宗教信仰 0.14 0.04 3.34*** [0.06, 0.23]
婚姻状况 -0.02 0.05 -0.35 [-0.12, 0.08]
客观阶层 0.08 0.06 1.31 [-0.04, 0.21]
内归因倾向 -0.15 0.04 -3.65*** [-0.23, -0.07]







[1] Alesina A., & Angeletos G. M.(2005). Fairness and redistribution. American Economic Review, 95(4), 960-980.
doi: 10.1257/0002828054825655URL
[2] Andersen R., & Curtis J.(2015). Social class, economic inequality, and the convergence of policy preferences: Evidence from 24 modern democracies. Canadian Review of Sociology, 52(3), 266-288.
doi: 10.1111/cars.2015.52.issue-3URL
[3] Belmi P., & Laurin K.(2016). Who wants to get to the top? Class and lay theories about power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(4), 505-529.
doi: 10.1037/pspi0000060URL
[4] Berg J. H., Stephan W. G., & Dodson M.(2006). Attributional modesty in women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 5(s5), 711-727.
[5] Blickle G., Diekmann C., Schneider P. B., Kalthöfer Y., & Summers J. K.(2012). When modesty wins: Impression management through modesty, political skill, and career success—A two-study investigation. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 21(6), 899-922.
doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2011.603900URL
[6] Brown-Iannuzzi J. L., Lundberg K. B., Kay A. C., & Payne B. K.(2015). Subjective status shapes political preferences. Psychological Science, 26(1), 15-26.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614553947pmid: 25416138
[7] Brown-Iannuzzi J. L., Lundberg K. B., & Mckee S.(2017). The politics of socioeconomic status: How socioeconomic status may influence political attitudes and engagement. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 11-14.
doi: S2352-250X(17)30072-6pmid: 29221505
[8] Bullock H. E.(2017). Social class and policy preferences: Implications for economic inequality and interclass relations. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 141-146.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.021URL
[9] Cai F.(2020). Creation and protection: Why do we need more redistribution. World Economics and Politics, 1, 5-21.
[ 蔡昉.(2020). 创造与保护: 为什么需要更多的再分配. 世界经济与政治, 1, 5-21.]
[10] Cohen S., Alper C. M., Doyle W. J., Adler N., Treanor J. J., & Turner R. B.(2008). Objective and subjective socioeconomic status and susceptibility to the common cold. Health Psychology, 27(2), 268-274.
doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.2.268URL
[11] Davis D. E., McElroy S., Choe E., Westbrook C. J., DeBlaere C., van Tongeren D. R.,... Placeres V.(2017). Development of the experiences of humility scale. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 45(1), 3-16.
doi: 10.1177/009164711704500101URL
[12] Davis D. E., Worthington E. L., & Hook J. N.(2010). Humility: Review of measurement strategies and conceptualization as personality judgment. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(4), 243-252.
doi: 10.1080/17439761003791672URL
[13] Durante R., Putterman L., & van der Weele J.(2014). Preferences for redistribution and perception of fairness: An experimental study. Journal of the European Economic Association, 12(4), 1059-1086.
doi: 10.1111/jeea.12082URL
[14] Exline J. J., Campbell W. K., Baumeister R. F., Joiner T., & Krueger J.(2004). Humility and modesty. In C. Peterson, & M. P. Seligman (Eds.), Character strengths and virtues: A handbook of classification (pp. 461-475). New York: Oxford University Press.
[15] Exline J. J., & Hill P. C.(2012). Humility: A consistent and robust predictor of generosity. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 7(3), 208-218.
doi: 10.1080/17439760.2012.671348URL
[16] Hayes A. F.(2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press.
[17] Hu X. Y., Guo Y. Y., Li J., & Yang S. L.(2016). Perceived societal fairness and goal attainment: The different effects of social class and their mechanism. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(3),271-289.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.00271URL
[ 胡小勇, 郭永玉, 李静, 杨沈龙.(2016). 社会公平感对不同阶层目标达成的影响及其过程. 心理学报, 48(3),271-289.]
[18] Hussak L. J., & Cimpian A.(2015). An early-emerging explanatory heuristic promotes support for the status quo. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(5), 739-752.
doi: 10.1037/pspa0000033URL
[19] Kraus M. W., & Callaghan B.(2014). Noblesse oblige? Social status and economic inequality maintenance among politicians. PLoS One, 9(1), e85293.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085293URL
[20] Kraus M. W., Côté S., & Keltner D.(2010). Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1716-1723.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610387613URL
[21] Kraus M. W., Piff P. K., & Keltner D.(2009). Social class, sense of control, and social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 992-1004.
doi: 10.1037/a0016357URL
[22] Krawczyk M.(2010). A glimpse through the veil of ignorance: Equality of opportunity and support for redistribution. Journal of Public Economics, 94(1-2), 131-141.
doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2009.10.003URL
[23] Kruse E., Chancellor J., & Lyubomirsky S.(2017). State humility: Measurement, conceptual validation, and intrapersonal processes. Self and Identity, 16(4), 399-438.
doi: 10.1080/15298868.2016.1267662URL
[24] Laurison D.(2016). Social class and political engagement in the United States. Sociology Compass, 10(8), 684-697.
doi: 10.1111/soc4.12390URL
[25] Leckelt M., Richter D., Schroder C., Kufner A. C., Grabka M. M., & Back M. D.(2019). The rich are different: Unravelling the perceived and self‐reported personality profiles of high‐net‐worth individuals. British Journal of Psychology, 110(4), 769-789.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12360pmid: 30466138
[26] Li J.(2014). Study on the tendency of attribution on the gap between the rich and the poor in different social classes. Guangzhou: World Publishing Corporation.
[ 李静.(2014). 不同社会阶层对贫富差距的心理归因研究. 广州: 世界图书出版公司.]
[27] Li Q. B.(2012). China’s income redistribution: Determinants of preferences, policy formation and effects calculation (Unpublished doctorial dissertation) . Nankai University, Tianjin, China.
[ 李清彬.(2012). 中国居民收入再分配的倾向决定, 政策形成和效应测算 (博士学位论文). 南开大学, 天津.]
[28] Li W. Q., Yang Y., Wu J. H., & Kou Y.(2020). Testing the status-legitimacy hypothesis in China: Objective and subjective socioeconomic status divergently predict system justification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(7), 1044-1058.
doi: 10.1177/0146167219893997URL
[29] Liu X.(2007). Class structure and the middle class location in urban China. Sociological Studies, 6, 1-14.
[ 刘欣.(2007). 中国城市的阶层结构与中产阶层的定位. 社会学研究, 6, 1-14.]
[30] Lu X. Y. (Ed.).(2002). Contemporary Chinese social class research report. Beijing,China: Social Sciences Academic Press.
[ 陆学艺.(Ed.).(2002). 当代中国社会阶层研究报告. 北京: 社会科学文献出版社.]
[31] Martin S. R., Côté S., & Woodruff T.(2016). Echoes of our upbringing: How growing up wealthy or poor relates to narcissism, leader behavior, and leader effectiveness. Academy of Management Journal, 59(6), 2157-2177.
doi: 10.5465/amj.2015.0680URL
[32] Meltzer A. H., & Richard S. F.(1981). A rational theory of the size of government. Journal of Political Economy, 89(5), 914-927.
doi: 10.1086/261013URL
[33] Miyamoto Y.(2017). Culture and social class. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 67-72.
doi: S2352-250X(17)30045-3pmid: 28826007
[34] Page B. I., Bartels L. M., & Seawright J.(2013). Democracy and the policy preferences of wealthy Americans. Perspectives on Politics, 11(1), 51-73.
doi: 10.1017/S153759271200360XURL
[35] Pan C. Y., & He L. X.(2011). Pursuing self interests or distributive justice? An empirical study of the preference for redistribution of Chinese residents. Journal of Economic Review, 5, 20-29.
[ 潘春阳, 何立新.(2011). 独善其身还是兼济天下?——中国居民再分配偏好的实证研究. 经济评论, 5, 20-29.]
[36] Piff P. K.(2014). Wealth and the inflated self: Class, entitlement, and narcissism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(1), 34-43.
doi: 10.1177/0146167213501699URL
[37] Piff P. K., Kraus M. W., Cote S., Cheng B. H., & Keltner D.(2010). Having less, giving more: The influence of social class on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 771-784.
doi: 10.1037/a0020092URL
[38] Piff P. K., Kraus M. W., & Keltner D.(2018). Unpacking the inequality paradox: The psychological roots of inequality and social class. In J. M. Olson (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 53-124). San Diego: Elsevier Academic Press.
[39] Qiu L., Zheng X., & Wang Y. F.(2008). Revision of the positive affect and negative affect scale. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 14(3), 249-254.
[ 邱林, 郑雪, 王雁飞.(2008). 积极情感消极情感量表(PANAS)的修订. 应用心理学, 14(3), 249-254.]
[40] Reeves A., & de Vries R.(2016). Does media coverage influence public attitudes towards welfare recipients? The impact of the 2011 English riots. British Journal of Sociology, 67(2), 281-306.
doi: 10.1111/bjos.2016.67.issue-2URL
[41] Rodriguez‐Bailon R., Bratanova B., Willis G. B., Lopez‐Rodriguez L., Sturrock A., & Loughnan S.(2017). Social class and ideologies of inequality: How they uphold unequal societies. Journal of Social Issues, 73(1), 99-116.
doi: 10.1111/josi.2017.73.issue-1URL
[42] Rodriguez‐Bailon R., Sanchez‐Rodriguez A., Garcia‐Sanchez E., Petkanopoulou K., & Willis G. B.(2020). Inequality is in the air: Contextual psychosocial effects of power and social class. Current Opinion in Psychology, 33, 120-125.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.004URL
[43] Sakurai K., Kawakami N., Yamaoka K., Ishikawa H., & Hashimoto H.(2010). The impact of subjective and objective social status on psychological distress among men and women in Japan. Social Science and Medicine, 70(11), 1832-1839.
doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.01.019pmid: 20303205
[44] Sandel M. J.(2018). Populism, liberalism, and democracy. Philosophy and Social Criticism, 44(4), 353-359.
doi: 10.1177/0191453718757888URL
[45] Sands M. L., & de Kadt D.(2020). Local exposure to inequality raises support of people of low wealth for taxing the wealthy. Nature, 586(7828), 257-261.
doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2763-1URL
[46] Tan J. J. X., & Kraus M. W.(2015). Lay theories about social class buffer lower-class individuals against poor self-rated health and negative affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(3), 446-461.
doi: 10.1177/0146167215569705URL
[47] Whitson J. A., & Galinsky A. D.(2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322(5898), 115-117.
doi: 10.1126/science.1159845URL
[48] Whyte M. K., & Han C.(2008). Popular attitudes toward distributive injustice: Beijing and Warsaw compared. Journal of Chinese Political Science, 13(1), 29-51.
doi: 10.1007/s11366-008-9016-8URL
[49] Worthington E. L., & Allison S. T.(2018). Heroic humility: What the science of humility can say to people raised on self-focus. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
[50] Wright J. C., Nadelhoffer T., Ross L. T., & Sinnott-Armstrong W.(2018). Be it ever so humble: Proposing a dual-dimension account and measurement of humility. Self and Identity, 17(1), 92-125.
doi: 10.1080/15298868.2017.1327454URL
[51] Xie W., Ho B., Meier S., & Zhou X.(2017). Rank reversal aversion inhibits redistribution across societies. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(8), 0142.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0142URL
[52] Xu J. B., & Liu H.(2013). Social justice recognition, fluidity expectation and the preference of residence redistribution: An empirical study based on CGSS data. Journal of Yunnan University of Finance and Economics, 2, 48-56.
[ 徐建斌, 刘华.(2013). 社会公平认知、流动性预期与居民再分配偏好——基于CGSS数据的实证研究. 云南财经大学学报 2, 48-56.]
[53] Xu J. B., Liu H., & Yin K. G.(2013). A research review about influence factors of residents’ redistributive preferences. Journal of Social Sciences Abroad, 2, 50-55.
[ 徐建斌, 刘华, 尹开国.(2013). 居民再分配偏好的影响因素研究述评. 国外社会科学, 2, 50-55.]
[54] Yang S. L., Guo Y. Y., Hu X. Y., Shu S. L., & Li J.(2016). Do lower class individuals possess higher levels of system justification? An examination from the social cognitive perspectives. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(11), 1467-1478.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.01467URL
[ 杨沈龙, 郭永玉, 胡小勇, 舒首立, 李静.(2016). 低阶层者的系统合理化水平更高吗?——基于社会认知视角的考察. 心理学报, 48(11), 1467-1478.]
[55] Yu G. L., Zhao F. Q., Wang H., & Li S.(2020). Subjective social class and distrust among Chinese college students: The mediating roles of relative deprivation and belief in a just world. Current Psychology, 39, 2221-2230.
doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-9908-5URL




[1]韦庆旺, 李木子, 陈晓晨. 社会阶层与社会知觉:热情和能力哪个更重要?[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(2): 243-252.
[2]刘圣明, 陈力凡, 王思迈. 满招损, 谦受益:团队沟通视角下谦卑型领导行为对团队创造力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(10): 1159-1168.
[3]杨林川, 马红宇, 姜海, 梁娟, 齐玲. 社会公正对权威合法性的影响: 社会阶层的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(7): 980-994.
[4]胡小勇;郭永玉;李静;杨沈龙. 社会公平感对不同阶层目标达成的影响及其过程[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(3): 271-289.
[5]杨沈龙;郭永玉;胡小勇; 舒首立;李静. 低阶层者的系统合理化水平更高吗? ——基于社会认知视角的考察[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(11): 1467-1478.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=5076
相关话题/政治 心理 户口 社会 统计