山东师范大学心理学院, 山东师范大学儿童青少年发展研究院, 济南 250014
收稿日期:
2021-05-20出版日期:
2021-09-25发布日期:
2021-07-22通讯作者:
曹衍淼E-mail:cym8023@126.com基金资助:
国家自然科学基金项目(32071073);国家自然科学基金项目(31900776);国家自然科学基金项目(31671156);教育部哲学社会科学研究重大课题攻关项目(17JZD058)The relationship between positive parenting and adolescent prosocial behaviour: The mediating role of empathy and the moderating role of the oxytocin receptor gene
ZHANG Wenxin, LI Xi, CHEN Guanghui, CAO Yanmiao()School of Psychology, Research Institute of Child and Adolescent Development, Shandong Normal University, Jinan 250014, China
Received:
2021-05-20Online:
2021-09-25Published:
2021-07-22Contact:
CAO Yanmiao E-mail:cym8023@126.com摘要/Abstract
摘要: 诸多研究显示母亲积极教养能够促进青少年亲社会行为的发展, 但是迄今对其内在作用机制尚不清楚。本研究基于“基因-环境-内表型-行为”模型, 采用问卷法和DNA分型技术, 对1082名青少年(初测年龄为12.32 ± 0.48岁, 50.3%女生)及其母亲进行为期2年的追踪研究, 考察共情在母亲积极教养与青少年亲社会行为间的中介作用, 以及OXTR基因rs53576多态性对该中介机制的调节作用。结果发现:(1)青少年认知共情(观点采择)在母亲积极教养和亲社会行为之间起中介作用, 而情绪共情(移情关心)的中介作用不显著; (2) OXTR基因对积极教养与亲社会行为间直接路径的调节作用不显著; (3) OXTR基因能够调节“母亲积极教养-认知共情-亲社会行为”中介机制的前半路径, 并且显现出一种超显性的基因效应。在携带GG和AA基因型的青少年中, 母亲积极教养显著正向预测认知共情, 进而增加了其亲社会行为, 而在携带AG杂合子基因型的青少年中, 这一中介效应则不显著。研究结果有助于从催产素受体基因多态性、共情能力和家庭教养等方面阐明青少年亲社会行为的发生机制及个体差异。
图/表 7
图1概念模型图
图1概念模型图
表1各变量间的相关系数
变量 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 SES | 1 | ||||||
2 性别 | 0.04 | 1 | |||||
3 积极教养(T1) | 0.17*** | -0.04 | 1 | ||||
4 认知共情(T2) | 0.03 | -0.04 | 0.11*** | 1 | |||
5 情绪共情(T2) | 0.01 | -0.15*** | 0.07* | 0.49*** | 1 | ||
6 亲社会行为(T1) | 0.15*** | -0.53*** | 0.14*** | 0.16*** | 0.22*** | 1 | |
7 亲社会行为(T2) | 0.04 | -0.49*** | 0.09*** | 0.14*** | 0.18*** | 0.53*** | 1 |
M (SD) | 0.00 (1.00) | — | 3.21 (0.52) | 2.94 (0.61) | 2.70 (0.64) | 1.76 (0.46) | 1.77 (0.48) |
表1各变量间的相关系数
变量 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 SES | 1 | ||||||
2 性别 | 0.04 | 1 | |||||
3 积极教养(T1) | 0.17*** | -0.04 | 1 | ||||
4 认知共情(T2) | 0.03 | -0.04 | 0.11*** | 1 | |||
5 情绪共情(T2) | 0.01 | -0.15*** | 0.07* | 0.49*** | 1 | ||
6 亲社会行为(T1) | 0.15*** | -0.53*** | 0.14*** | 0.16*** | 0.22*** | 1 | |
7 亲社会行为(T2) | 0.04 | -0.49*** | 0.09*** | 0.14*** | 0.18*** | 0.53*** | 1 |
M (SD) | 0.00 (1.00) | — | 3.21 (0.52) | 2.94 (0.61) | 2.70 (0.64) | 1.76 (0.46) | 1.77 (0.48) |
表2共情在积极教养与亲社会行为间的中介模型拟合指数
Model | χ2/df | CFI | TLI | RMSEA (90% CI) | Path a | Path b | Path c | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
认知共情 | 6.50 | 0.91 | 0.90 | 0.072[0.069~0.075] | 0.14*** | 0.14*** | 0.08* | [0.006, 0.028] |
认知共情(控制T1亲社会) | 6.41 | 0.91 | 0.90 | 0.071[0.068~0.074] | 0.14*** | 0.09** | 0.04 | [0.001, 0.020] |
情绪共情 | 7.49 | 0.89 | 0.88 | 0.078[0.075~0.081] | 0.09* | 0.14*** | 0.08*. | [-0.001, 0.022] |
情绪共情(控制T1亲社会) | 7.36 | 0.88 | 0.87 | 0.077[0.074~0.080] | 0.09+ | 0.07+ | 0.04* | [-0.002, 0.013] |
表2共情在积极教养与亲社会行为间的中介模型拟合指数
Model | χ2/df | CFI | TLI | RMSEA (90% CI) | Path a | Path b | Path c | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
认知共情 | 6.50 | 0.91 | 0.90 | 0.072[0.069~0.075] | 0.14*** | 0.14*** | 0.08* | [0.006, 0.028] |
认知共情(控制T1亲社会) | 6.41 | 0.91 | 0.90 | 0.071[0.068~0.074] | 0.14*** | 0.09** | 0.04 | [0.001, 0.020] |
情绪共情 | 7.49 | 0.89 | 0.88 | 0.078[0.075~0.081] | 0.09* | 0.14*** | 0.08*. | [-0.001, 0.022] |
情绪共情(控制T1亲社会) | 7.36 | 0.88 | 0.87 | 0.077[0.074~0.080] | 0.09+ | 0.07+ | 0.04* | [-0.002, 0.013] |
表3有调节的中介模型(四种基因编码方式)
变量 | 模型1 (因变量:亲社会行为) | 模型2 (因变量:认知共情) | 模型3 (因变量:亲社会行为) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ΔR2 | b | SE | ΔR2 | b | SE | ΔR2 | b | SE | |
第一层:SES | 0.24*** | 0.03* | 0.01 | 0.003 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.24*** | 0.03* | 0.01 |
性别 | -0.47*** | 0.03 | -0.04 | 0.04 | -0.47*** | 0.03 | |||
第二层:积极教养 | 0.01** | 0.03* | 0.01 | 0.01*** | 0.07*** | 0.02 | 0.01** | 0.03* | 0.01 |
第三层:认知共情 | 0.01*** | 0.06*** | 0.01 | — | — | — | 0.01*** | 0.06*** | 0.01 |
第四层:OXTR基因 | |||||||||
虚拟编码:AG vs. AA | 0.002 | 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.001 | 0.01 | 0.03 | |||
GG vs. AA | -0.03 | 0.07 | -0.04 | 0.05 | |||||
加性编码:OXTR | 0.000 | 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.00 | -0.01 | 0.02 | |||
显性编码:OXTR | 0.001 | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.002 | 0.03 | |||
隐性编码:OXTR | 0.001 | -0.05 | 0.07 | 0.001 | -0.05 | 0.04 | |||
第五层:OXTR×积极教养 | |||||||||
虚拟编码:积极教养×AG/AA | 0.01** | -0.11** | 0.04 | 0.001 | 0.02 | 0.03 | |||
积极教养×GG/AA | 0.03 | 0.07 | -0.02 | 0.05 | |||||
加性编码:积极教养×OXTR | 0.001 | -0.03 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.003 | 0.02 | |||
显性编码:积极教养×OXTR | 0.004 | -0.08* | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.03 | |||
隐性编码:积极教养×OXTR | 0.001 | 0.08 | 0.07 | 0.00 | -0.03 | 0.05 |
表3有调节的中介模型(四种基因编码方式)
变量 | 模型1 (因变量:亲社会行为) | 模型2 (因变量:认知共情) | 模型3 (因变量:亲社会行为) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ΔR2 | b | SE | ΔR2 | b | SE | ΔR2 | b | SE | |
第一层:SES | 0.24*** | 0.03* | 0.01 | 0.003 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.24*** | 0.03* | 0.01 |
性别 | -0.47*** | 0.03 | -0.04 | 0.04 | -0.47*** | 0.03 | |||
第二层:积极教养 | 0.01** | 0.03* | 0.01 | 0.01*** | 0.07*** | 0.02 | 0.01** | 0.03* | 0.01 |
第三层:认知共情 | 0.01*** | 0.06*** | 0.01 | — | — | — | 0.01*** | 0.06*** | 0.01 |
第四层:OXTR基因 | |||||||||
虚拟编码:AG vs. AA | 0.002 | 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.001 | 0.01 | 0.03 | |||
GG vs. AA | -0.03 | 0.07 | -0.04 | 0.05 | |||||
加性编码:OXTR | 0.000 | 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.00 | -0.01 | 0.02 | |||
显性编码:OXTR | 0.001 | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.002 | 0.03 | |||
隐性编码:OXTR | 0.001 | -0.05 | 0.07 | 0.001 | -0.05 | 0.04 | |||
第五层:OXTR×积极教养 | |||||||||
虚拟编码:积极教养×AG/AA | 0.01** | -0.11** | 0.04 | 0.001 | 0.02 | 0.03 | |||
积极教养×GG/AA | 0.03 | 0.07 | -0.02 | 0.05 | |||||
加性编码:积极教养×OXTR | 0.001 | -0.03 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.003 | 0.02 | |||
显性编码:积极教养×OXTR | 0.004 | -0.08* | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.03 | |||
隐性编码:积极教养×OXTR | 0.001 | 0.08 | 0.07 | 0.00 | -0.03 | 0.05 |
图2OXTR各基因型中母亲积极教养与认知共情的相关散点图
图2OXTR各基因型中母亲积极教养与认知共情的相关散点图
图3 OXTR基因rs53576多态性与积极教养对青少年认知共情的影响
图3 OXTR基因rs53576多态性与积极教养对青少年认知共情的影响
表4有调节的中介效应的Bootstrap分析
效应 | 子样本1 | 子样本2 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effect | BootSE | 95% CI | Effect | BootSE | 95% CI | |
中介效应 | ||||||
AA | 0.01 | 0.004 | [0.002, 0.02] | 0.01 | 0.01 | [0.004, 0.03] |
AG | -0.003 | 0.004 | [-0.01, 0.01] | 0.003 | 0.004 | [-0.01, 0.01] |
GG | 0.02 | 0.01 | [0.002, 0.04] | 0.01 | 0.01 | [-0.002, 0.03] |
有调节的中介效应 | ||||||
AG vs. AA | -0.01 | 0.01 | [-0.02, 0.000] | -0.01 | 0.01 | [-0.03, -0.0001] |
GG vs. AA | 0.01 | 0.01 | [-0.002, 0.03] | -0.003 | 0.01 | [-0.02, 0.01] |
表4有调节的中介效应的Bootstrap分析
效应 | 子样本1 | 子样本2 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effect | BootSE | 95% CI | Effect | BootSE | 95% CI | |
中介效应 | ||||||
AA | 0.01 | 0.004 | [0.002, 0.02] | 0.01 | 0.01 | [0.004, 0.03] |
AG | -0.003 | 0.004 | [-0.01, 0.01] | 0.003 | 0.004 | [-0.01, 0.01] |
GG | 0.02 | 0.01 | [0.002, 0.04] | 0.01 | 0.01 | [-0.002, 0.03] |
有调节的中介效应 | ||||||
AG vs. AA | -0.01 | 0.01 | [-0.02, 0.000] | -0.01 | 0.01 | [-0.03, -0.0001] |
GG vs. AA | 0.01 | 0.01 | [-0.002, 0.03] | -0.003 | 0.01 | [-0.02, 0.01] |
参考文献 84
[1] | Aliev F., Latendresse S.J., Bacanu S.A., Neale M.C., & Dick D.M. (2014). Testing for measured gene-environment interaction: Problems with the use of cross-product terms and a regression model reparameterization solution. Behavior Genetics, 44(2), 165-181. doi: 10.1007/s10519-014-9642-1URL |
[2] | Augustine M.E., & Stifter C.A. (2015). Temperament, parenting, and moral development: Specificity of behavior and context. Social Development, 24(2), 285-303. doi: 10.1111/sode.12092pmid: 28670101 |
[3] | Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J., & van Ijzendoorn M.H. (2014). A sociability gene? Meta-analysis of oxytocin receptor genotype effects in humans. Psychiatric Genetics, 24(2), 45-51. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283643684pmid: 23921259 |
[4] | Bartz J.A., Zaki J., Bolger N., & Ochsner K.N. (2011). Social effects of oxytocin in humans: Context and person matter. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 301-309. |
[5] | Batson C.D., Eklund J.H., Chermok V.L., Hoyt J.L., & Ortiz B.G. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 65-74. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.65URL |
[6] | Bradley B., Davis T.A., Wingo A.P., Mercer K.B., & Ressler K.J. (2013). Family environment and adult resilience: Contributions of positive parenting and the oxytocin receptor gene. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 4(1), 21659. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.21659URL |
[7] | Cao C., Cao L., & Chen J. (2019). Differences in sensitivity to environment depending on Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) Gene? A meta-analysis of child and adolescent gene-by-environment studies. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48(4), 655-667. doi: 10.1007/s10964-019-01004-3URL |
[8] | Cao C., Rijlaarsdam J., van der Voort A., Ji L., Zhang W., & Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J. (2018). Associations between dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) gene, maternal positive parenting and trajectories of depressive symptoms from early to mid-adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(2), 365-379. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0294-5URL |
[9] | Carlo G., White R.M.B., Streit C., Knight G.P., & Zeiders K.H. (2017). Longitudinal relations among parenting styles, prosocial behaviors, and academic outcomes in U.S. Mexican adolescents. Child Development, 89(2), 577-592. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12761URL |
[10] | Caspi A., & Moffitt T.E. (2006). Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: Joining forces with neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(7), 583-590. |
[11] | Chen B., Zhu Z., Wang Y., Ding X., Guo X., He M.,... Rao Y. (2018). Nature vs. nurture in human sociality: Multi- level genomic analyses of social conformity. Journal of Human Genetics, 63(5), 605-619. doi: 10.1038/s10038-018-0418-yURL |
[12] | Chen F.S., Kumsta R., von Dawans B., Monakhov M., Ebstein R.P., & Heinrichs M. (2011). Common oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism and social support interact to reduce stress in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(50), 19937-19942. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113079108URL |
[13] | Chen L., Zhang W.X., Ji L., & Deater-Deckard K. (2019). Developmental trajectories of Chinese adolescents’ relational aggression: Associations with changes in social-psychological adjustment. Child Development, 90(6), 2153-2170. doi: 10.1111/cdev.v90.6URL |
[14] | Chen X., Bian Y., Xin T., Wang L., & Silbereisen R.K. (2010). Perceived social change and childrearing attitudes in China. European Psychologist, 15(4), 260-270. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000060URL |
[15] | Chen X., McCormick E.M., Ravindran N., McElwain N.L., & Telzer E.H. (2020). Maternal emotion socialization in early childhood predicts adolescents’ amygdala-vmPFC functional connectivity to emotion faces. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 503-515. doi: 10.1037/dev0000852pmid: 32077720 |
[16] | Comings D.E., & MacMurray J.P. (2000). Molecular heterosis: A review. Molecular Genetics and Metabolism, 71(1-2), 19-31. doi: 10.1006/mgme.2000.3048URL |
[17] | Costa B., Pini S., Gabelloni P., Abelli M., Lari L., Cardini A., … Martini C. (2009). Oxytocin receptor polymorphisms and adult attachment style in patients with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(10), 1506-1514. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.006URL |
[18] | Davis A.N., & Carlo G. (2018). The roles of parenting practices, sociocognitive/emotive traits, and prosocial behaviors in low-income adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 62, 140-150. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.11.011URL |
[19] | Davis M.H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10(85) |
[20] | Decety J., Bartal I. B-A., Uzefovsky F., & Knafo-Noam A. (2015). Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: Highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1686), 20150077. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0077URL |
[21] | de Waal F.B.M., & Preston, S.D. (2017). Mammalian empathy: Behavioural manifestations and neural basis. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(8), 498-509. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.72URL |
[22] | Dick D.M., Agrawal A., Keller M.C., Adkins A., Aliev F., Monroe S., … Sher K.J. (2015). Candidate gene- environment interaction research: Reflections and recommendations. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 37-59. doi: 10.1177/1745691614556682URL |
[23] | Duncan L.E., Pollastri A.R., & Smoller J.W. (2014). Mind the gap: Why many geneticists and psychological scientists have discrepant views about gene-environment interaction (G×E) research. American Psychologist, 69(3), 249-268. doi: 10.1037/a0036320URL |
[24] | Eisenberg N., Spinrad T.L., & Knafo-Noam A. (2015). Prosocial development. In M.E. Lamb, & C. Garcia Coll (Vol. Eds.), R.M. Lerner (Series Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (7th ed., pp. 610-656). New York: Wiley. |
[25] | Fang Y., Li Z., Wu S., Wang C., Dong Y., & He S. (2020). Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms moderate the relationship between job stress and general trust in Chinese Han university teachers. Journal of Affective Disorders, 260, 18-23. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.08.080URL |
[26] | Fan X.H., Fang X.Y., Liu Y., Lin X.Y., & Yuan X.J. (2012). The effect of social support and social identity on the relationship between perceived discrimination and socio-cultural adjustment among Chinese migrant children. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(5), 647-663. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2012.00647URL |
[ 范兴华, 方晓义, 刘杨, 蔺秀云, 袁晓娇. (2012). 流动儿童歧视知觉与社会文化适应: 社会支持和社会认同的作用. 心理学报, 44(5), 647-663.] | |
[27] | Feng C., Lori A., Waldman I.D., Binder E.B., Haroon E., & Rilling J.K. (2015). A common oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism modulates intranasal oxytocin effects on the neural response to social cooperation in humans. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 14(7), 516-525. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12234URL |
[28] | Field A.P. (2005). Intraclass correlation. In B. S. Everitt & D. C. Howell (Eds.), Encyclopedia of statistics in the behavioral sciences (pp.1296-1305). Chichester, England: Wiley. |
[29] | * Flasbeck V., Moser D., Kumsta R., & Brüne M. (2018). The OXTR single-nucleotide polymorphism rs53576 moderates the impact of childhood maltreatment on empathy for social pain in female participants: Evidence for differential susceptibility. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 359. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00359pmid: 30135663 |
[30] | Fortuna K., & Knafo A. (2014). Parental and genetic contributions to prosocial behavior during childhood. In L.M. Padilla-Walker & G. Carlo (Eds.), Prosocial development: A multidimensional approach (pp.70-89). Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[31] | Gong P., Fan H., Liu J., Yang X., Zhang K., & Zhou X. (2017). Revisiting the impact of OXTR rs53576 on empathy: A population-based study and a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 80, 131-136. |
[32] | Grusec J.E., & Goodnow J.J. (1994). Impact of parental discipline methods on the child's internalization of values: A reconceptualization of current points of view. Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 4-19. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.1.4URL |
[33] | Hackel L.M., Zaki J., & van Bavel J.J. (2017). Social identity shapes social valuation: Evidence from prosocial behavior and vicarious reward. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(8), 1219-1228. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx045pmid: 28402506 |
[34] | Hastings P.D., McShane K.E., Parker R., & Ladha F. (2007). Ready to make nice: Parental socialization of young sons’ and daughters’ prosocial behaviors with peers. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 168(2), 177-200. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.168.2.177-200URL |
[35] | Hastings P., Miller J., & Troxel N.R. (2015). Making good:The socialization of children’s prosocial development. In J. Grusec & P.D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research (pp.637-666). New York, NY: Guilford Press. |
[36] | Hayes A.F. (2013). An introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press. |
[37] | He J., Buil J.M., Koot H.M., & van Lier P.A.C. (2018). Associations between oxytocin receptor (OXTR) genotype and elementary school children’s likability, dis-likability and friendship among classroom peers: A longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47(9), 1799-1812. doi: 10.1007/s10964-018-0855-0URL |
[38] | Hoffman M.L. (1970). Moral development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child development (Vol. 2, pp. 261-359). New York: Wiley. |
[39] | Hoffman M.L. (1983). Affective and cognitive processes in moral internalization. In E. T. Higgins, D.N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development (pp.236-274). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. |
[40] | Hoffman M.L. (2001). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press. |
[41] | Hostinar C.E., Cicchetti D., & Rogosch F.A. (2014). Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism, perceived social support, and psychological symptoms in maltreated adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 26(2), 465-477. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000066pmid: 24621832 |
[42] | Huang Y.Q., & Su Y.J. (2012). The development of empathy across the lifespan: A perspective of double processes. Psychological Development and Education, 28(4), 434- 441. |
[ 黄翯青, 苏彦捷. (2012). 共情的毕生发展: 一个双过程的视角. 心理发展与教育, 28(4), 434-441.] | |
[43] | Kennedy D., & Norman C. (2005). What don’t we know? Science, 309(5731), 75. pmid: 15994521 |
[44] | Kiang L., Moreno A.J., & Robinson J.L. (2004). Maternal preconceptions about parenting predict child temperament, maternal sensitivity, and children’s empathy. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1081-1092. pmid: 15535758 |
[45] | Kim E.K., You S., & Knox J. (2019). The mediating effect of empathy on the relation between child self-expressiveness in family and prosocial behaviors. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 1-10. |
[46] | Kim H.S., Sherman D.K., Sasaki J.Y., Xu J., Chu T.Q., Ryu C.,... Taylor S.E. (2010). Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(36), 15717-15721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010830107URL |
[47] | Knafo A., Israel S., & Ebstein R.P. (2011). Heritability of children's prosocial behavior and differential susceptibility to parenting by variation in the dopamine receptor D4 gene. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 53-67. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000647URL |
[48] | Knafo-Noam A., Vertsberger D., & Israel S. (2018). Genetic and environmental contributions to children's prosocial behavior: Brief review and new evidence from a reanalysis of experimental twin data. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 60-65. doi: S2352-250X(17)30162-8pmid: 28858769 |
[49] | Kopala-Sibley D.C., Cyr M., Finsaas M.C., Orawe J., Huang A., Tottenham N., & Klein D.N. (2018). Early childhood parenting predicts late childhood brain functional connectivity during emotion perception and reward processing. Child Development, 91(1), 110-128. doi: 10.1111/cdev.v91.1URL |
[50] | Lim D., & DeSteno D. (2016). Suffering and compassion: The links among adverse life experiences, empathy, compassion, and prosocial behavior. Emotion, 16(2), 175- 182. doi: 10.1037/emo0000144URL |
[51] | Luo S., Li B., Ma Y., Zhang W., Rao Y., & Han S. (2015). Oxytocin receptor gene and racial ingroup bias in empathy-related brain activity. NeuroImage, 110, 22-31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.042URL |
[52] | Luo S., Zhang T., Li W., Yu M., Hein G., & Han S. (2019). Interactions between oxytocin receptor gene and intergroup relationship on empathic neural responses to others’ pain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(5), 505- 517. |
[53] | Masten C.L., Morelli S.A., & Eisenberger N.I. (2011). An fMRI investigation of empathy for ‘social pain’ and subsequent prosocial behavior. NeuroImage, 55(1), 381- 388. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.060URL |
[54] | Mcdonald N.M., Baker J.K., & Messinger D.S. (2016). Oxytocin and parent-child interaction in the development of empathy among children at risk for autism. Developmental Psychology, 52(5), 735-745. doi: 10.1037/dev0000104URL |
[55] | McQuaid R.J., McInnis O.A., Stead J.D., Matheson K., & Anisman H. (2013). A paradoxical association of an oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism: Early-life adversity and vulnerability to depression. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 128. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00128pmid: 23898235 |
[56] | Meyer-Lindenberg A., & Weinberger D.R. (2006). Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(10), 818-827. pmid: 16988657 |
[57] | Moons W.G., Way B.M., & Taylor S.E. (2014). Oxytocin and vasopressin receptor polymorphisms interact with circulating neuropeptides to predict human emotional reactions to stress. Emotion, 14(3), 562-572. doi: 10.1037/a0035503URL |
[58] | Newton E.K., Laible D., Carlo G., Steele J.S., & McGinley M. (2014). Do sensitive parents foster kind children, or vice versa? Bidirectional influences between children’s prosocial behavior and parental sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 50(6), 1808-1816. doi: 10.1037/a0036495pmid: 24708456 |
[59] | Padilla-Walker L.M., Carlo G., Christensen K.J., & Yorgason J.B. (2012). Bidirectional relations between authoritative parenting and adolescents’ prosocial behaviors. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 22(3), 400-408. doi: 10.1111/jora.2012.22.issue-3URL |
[60] | Padilla-Walker L.M., & Christensen K.J. (2011). Empathy and self‐regulation as mediators between parenting and adolescents' prosocial behavior toward strangers, friends, and family. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21(3), 545-551. doi: 10.1111/jora.2011.21.issue-3URL |
[61] | Padilla-Walker L.M., Nielson M.G., & Day R.D. (2016). The role of parental warmth and hostility on adolescents’ prosocial behavior toward multiple targets. Journal of Family Psychology, 30(3), 331-340. doi: 10.1037/fam0000157pmid: 26414417 |
[62] | Reiner I., Frieling H., Beutel M., & Michal M. (2016). Gene-environment interaction of the oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (rs53576) and unresolved attachment status predict depersonalization symptoms: An exploratory study. Psychological Studies, 61(4), 295-300. |
[63] | Rilling J.K., DeMarco A.C., Hackett P.D., Chen X., Gautam P., Stair S.,... Pagnoni G. (2014). Sex differences in the neural and behavioral response to intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin during human social interaction. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 39, 237-248. doi: S0306-4530(13)00341-7pmid: 24157401 |
[64] | Poulin M.J., Holman E.A., & Buffone A. (2012). The neurogenetics of nice: Receptor genes for oxytocin and vasopressin interact with threat to predict prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 23(5), 446-452. |
[65] | Shamay-Tsoory S.G., & Abu-Akel A. (2016). The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biological Psychiatry, 79(3), 194-202. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020pmid: 26321019 |
[66] | Singer T. (2006). The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future research. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(6), 855-863. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.011URL |
[67] | Siu A.M.H., & Shek D.T.L. (2005). Validation of the interpersonal reactivity index in a Chinese context. Research on Social Work Practice, 15(2), 118-126. doi: 10.1177/1049731504270384URL |
[68] | Smearman E.L., Winiarski D.A., Brennan P.A., Najman J., & Johnson K.C. (2015). Social stress and the oxytocin receptor gene interact to predict antisocial behavior in an at-risk cohort. Development and Psychopathology, 27(1), 309-318. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000649pmid: 25003328 |
[69] | Starr L.R., Hammen C., Conway C.C., Raposa E., & Brennan P.A. (2014). Sensitizing effect of early adversity on depressive reactions to later proximal stress: Moderation by polymorphisms in serotonin transporter and corticotropin releasing hormone receptor genes in a 20-year longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4), 1241- 1254. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000996URL |
[70] | Steinberg L., Lamborn S.D., Dornbusch S.M., & Darling N. (1992). Impact of parenting practices on adolescent achievement: Authoritative parenting, school involvement, and encouragement to succeed. Child Development, 63(5), 1266-1281. pmid: 1446552 |
[71] | Tost H., Kolachana B., Hakimi S., Lemaitre H., Verchinski B.A., Mattay V.S., … Meyer-Lindenberg A. (2010). A common allele in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human hypothalamic- limbic structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(31), 13936-13941. |
[72] | Uzefovsky F., Shalev I., Israel S., Edelman S., Raz Y., Mankuta D., … Ebstein R.P. (2015). Oxytocin receptor and vasopressin receptor 1a genes are respectively associated with emotional and cognitive empathy. Hormones and Behavior, 67, 60-65. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.11.007pmid: 25476609 |
[73] | van Roekel, E., Verhagen, M., Scholte, R.H.J., Kleinjan, M., Goossens, L., & Engels, R.C.M.E. (2013). The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in relation to state levels of loneliness in adolescence: Evidence for micro-level gene-environment interactions. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e77689. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077689URL |
[74] | Vrshek-Schallhorn S., Stroud C.B., Mineka S., Zinbarg R.E., Adam E.K., Redei E.E.,... Craske M.G. (2015). Additive genetic risk from five serotonin system polymorphisms interacts with interpersonal stress to predict depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(4), 776-790. |
[75] | Wang S.Q., Zhang W.X., Chen L., Li H.L., Li C., & Zhou L.N. (2011). A multitrait-multimethod analysis of aggressive behaviors in middle childhood. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 43(3), 294-307. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2011.00294URL |
[ 王姝琼, 张文新, 陈亮, 李海垒, 李春, 周利娜. (2011). 儿童中期攻击行为测评的多质多法分析. 心理学报, 43(3), 294-307.] | |
[76] | Wen Z.L., & Ye B.J. (2014). Different methods for testing moderated mediation models: Competitors or backups? Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(5), 714-726. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00714URL |
[ 温忠麟, 叶宝娟. (2014). 有调节的中介模型检验方法:竞争还是替补? 心理学报, 46(5), 714-726.] | |
[77] | Wu N., & Su Y. (2015). Oxytocin receptor gene relates to theory of mind and prosocial behavior in children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(2), 302-313. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.858042URL |
[78] | Yamasue H. (2013). Function and structure in social brain regions can link oxytocin-receptor genes with autistic social behavior. Brain and Development, 35(2), 111-118. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2012.08.010URL |
[79] | Yan Z.Q., & Su Y.J. (2021). Difference between cognitive empathy and affective empathy in development: Meta- analysis preliminary exploration. Psychological Development and Education, 37(1), 1-9. doi: 10.1017/edp.2020.6URL |
[ 颜志强, 苏彦捷. (2021). 认知共情和情绪共情的发展差异: 元分析初探. 心理发展与教育, 37(1), 1-9.] | |
[80] | Yorgason J.B., & Gustafson K.B. (2014). Linking grandparent involvement with the development of prosocial behavior in adolescents. In L. M. Padilla-Walker & G. Carlo (Eds.) Prosocial development: A multidimensional approach (pp.201-217). Oxford Scholarship Online. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199964772.003.0010 doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199964772.003.0010 |
[81] | * Yu X. Q., Dong Y., Li Z. N., Fang Y., Wu S., Wang C., & He S. C. (2020). Work stress and general trust: The mediating effect of depression and the moderating effect of the OXTR gene rs53576. Journal of Affective Disorders, 272, 283-288. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.160URL |
[82] | Zhang F.F., Dong Y., Wang K., Zhan Z.Y., & Xie L.F. (2010). Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index-C. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 18(2), 155-157. |
[ 张凤凤, 董毅, 汪凯, 詹志禹, 谢伦芳. (2010). 中文版人际反应指针量表(IRI-C)的信度及效度研究. 中国临床心理学杂志, 18(2), 155-157.] | |
[83] | Zhang W., Wei X., Ji L., Chen L., & Deater-Deckard K. (2017). Reconsidering parenting in Chinese culture: Subtypes, stability, and change of maternal parenting style during early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46(5), 1117-1136. doi: 10.1007/s10964-017-0664-xURL |
[84] | * Zheng S., Masuda T., Matsunaga M., Noguchi Y., Ohtsubo Y., Yamasue H., & Ishii K. (2020). Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and childhood adversity influence trust. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 121, 104840. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104840URL |
相关文章 9
[1] | 王文超, 伍新春. 共情对灾后青少年亲社会行为的影响:感恩、社会支持和创伤后成长的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(3): 307-316. |
[2] | 姚琦, 吴章建, 张常清, 符国群. 权力感对炫耀性亲社会行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(12): 1421-1435. |
[3] | 王建峰, 戴冰. “追名弃利”:权力动机与社会存在对亲社会行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 55-65. |
[4] | 王艳辉;李董平;孙文强;赵力燕;赖雪芬;周月月. 亲子依恋与初中生亲社会行为:有调节的中介效应[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(5): 663-679. |
[5] | 丁如一;周晖;张豹;陈晓. 自恋与青少年亲社会行为之间的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(8): 981-988. |
[6] | 王栋;陈作松. 运动员运动道德推脱与运动亲反社会行为的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(3): 305-317. |
[7] | 郑晓莹;彭泗清;彭璐珞. “达”则兼济天下?社会比较对亲社会行为的影响及心理机制[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(2): 243-250. |
[8] | 马伟娜;朱蓓蓓. 孤独症儿童的情绪共情能力及情绪表情注意方式[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(4): 528-539. |
[9] | 赵景欣,张文新,纪林芹. 幼儿二级错误信念认知、亲社会行为与同伴接纳的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2005, 37(06): 760-766. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=5040