删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

仪式动作、象征意义和积极情绪增强控制感:双路径机制

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

尹可丽, 兰淼森, 李慧, 赵子文
云南师范大学教育学部, 昆明 650500
收稿日期:2021-01-05出版日期:2022-01-25发布日期:2021-11-26
通讯作者:尹可丽, E-mail: yayasles@163.com

基金资助:* 教育部人文社会科学研究规划基金项目(19YJAZH103)、国家自然科学基金地区项目(72061012)资助

How ritual actions, symbolic meanings, and positive emotions enhance perceived control: A dual path way mechanism

YIN Keli, LAN Miaosen, LI Hui, ZHAO Ziwen
Faculty of Education, Yunnan Normal University, Kunming 650500, China
Received:2021-01-05Online:2022-01-25Published:2021-11-26







摘要/Abstract


摘要: 以中国普米族人的传统文化仪式“敬锅庄”为例, 采用回忆任务、创设新颖仪式等方法考察仪式动作、象征意义和积极情绪对普米族青少年及成人的控制感的影响。结果表明, 熟悉敬锅庄仪式的动作、象征意义或者具有更多情感体验的青少年的控制感更强。仪式动作与象征意义对控制感的影响存在双路径机制:仪式动作直接增强个体的控制感, 象征意义通过积极情绪间接增强个体的控制感。象征意义与控制感的关系还因仪式主体不同有所区别:祈求庇佑通过积极情绪间接增强青少年的控制感, 表达感恩通过积极情绪间接增强成年人的控制感。研究结果对探究仪式动作、象征意义和积极情绪对个体控制感的影响有重要启示。


[1] Adler, M. G., & Fagley, N. S. (2005). Appreciation: Individual differences in finding value and meaning as a unique predictor of subjective well-being.Journal of Personality, 73(1), 79-114.
[2] Alonso-Ferres M., Imami L., & Slatcher R. B. (2019). Untangling the effects of partner responsiveness on health and well-being: The role of perceived control.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 37(4), 1150-1171.
[3] Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals.Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 29(6), 595-613.
[4] Brooks A. W., Schroeder J., Risen J. L., Gino F., Galinsky A. D., Norton M. I., & Schweitzer M. E. (2016). Don’t stop believing: Rituals improve performance by decreasing anxiety.Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 137, 71-85.
[5] Chun, J. W., & Lee, M. J. (2017). When does individuals’ willingness to speak out increase on social media? Perceived social support and perceived power/control. Computers in Human Behavior, 74, 120-129.
[6] Crespo C., Kielpikowski M., Pryor J., & Jose P. E. (2011). Family rituals in New Zealand families: Links to family cohesion and adolescents' well-being. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(2), 184-193.
[7] DeSteno D., Li Y., Dickens L., & Lerner J. S. (2014). Gratitude: A tool for reducing economic impatience.Psychological Science, 25(6), 1262-1267.
[8] Douglas, M. (2020). Purity and Dange: An analysis of concept of pollution and taboo (Huang J. B., & Liu B. Y., Lu, C.). Beijing: The Commercial Press. (Original work published 1966)
[玛丽∙道格拉斯. (2020). 洁净与危险——对污染和禁忌观念的分析 (黄剑波, 柳博赟, 卢忱译). 北京: 商务印书馆.]
[9] Drewelies J., Schade H., Hülür G., Hoppmann C. A., Ram N., & Gerstorf D. (2018). The more we are in control, the merrier? Partner perceived control and negative affect in the daily lives of older couples.Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75(2), 338-348.
[10] Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.
[11] Fischer R., Callander R., Reddish P., & Bulbulia J. (2013). How do rituals affect cooperation?Human Nature, 24(2), 115-125.
[12] Gallagher, T. J., & Lewis, J. M. (2010). Rationalists, fatalists, and the modern superstitious: Test‐taking in introductory sociology.Sociological Inquiry, 71(1), 1-12.
[13] Greenaway K. H., Haslam S. A., Cruwys T., Branscombe N. R., Ysseldyk R., & Heldreth C. (2015). From "we" to "me": Group identification enhances perceived personal control with consequences for health and well-being.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 53-74.
[14] Han J. J., Gershoff A. D., Kirmani A., & Dalton A. (2018). When good things feel closer and bad things feel farther: The role of perceived control on psychological distance perception. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28(4), 629-643.
[15] Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression- based approach. New York: Guilford.
[16] Hobson N. M., Bonk D., & Inzlicht M. (2017). Rituals decrease the neural response to performance failure.Peerj, 5(4), e3363.
[17] Hobson N. M., Gino F., Norton M. I., & Inzlicht, M. (2016). Novel rituals inculcate intergroup bias: Evidence from the trust game and neurophysiology. Social Science Electronic Publishing.
[18] Hobson N. M., Schroeder J., Risen J. L., Xygalatas D., & Inzlicht M. (2018). The psychology of rituals: An integrative review and process-based framework.Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(3), 260-284.
[19] Infurna F. J., Gerstorf D., Ram N., Schupp J., & Wagner G. G. (2011). Long-term antecedents and outcomes of perceived control. Psychology and Aging, 26(3), 559.
[20] Kapitány, R., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Adopting the ritual stance: The role of opacity and context in ritual and everyday actions. Cognition, 145, 13-29.
[21] Kapitány, R., & Nielsen, M. (2017). The ritual stance and the precaution system: The role of goal-demotion and opacity in ritual and everyday actions.Religion, Brain & Behavior, 7(1), 27-42.
[22] Legare, C. H., & Souza, A. L. (2012). Evaluating ritual efficacy: Evidence from the supernatural.Cognition, 124(1), 1-15.
[23] Legare, C. H., & Souza, A. L. (2014). Searching for control: Priming randomness increases the evaluation of ritual efficacy.Cognitive Science, 38(1), 152-161.
[24] Legare C. H., Wen N. J., Herrmann P. A., & Whitehouse H. (2015). Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning.Cognition, 142, 351-361.
[25] Lin, C. C. (2019). Gratitude, positive emotion, and satisfaction with life: A test of mediated effect.Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 47(4), 1-8.
[26] Long, X. T. (2019). The performance and connotation of generational “Death Education” and its modern value. Religious Studies, 4, 269-275.
[龙晓添. (2019). 世代生成式“死亡教育”的展演、内涵及其现代价值.宗教学研究, 4, 269-275]
[27] Malinowski, B. (1948). Magic, science and religion and other essays. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
[28] Martin L., Jan K., Shaver J. H., Danijela J., & Dimitris X. (2015). Effects of anxiety on spontaneous ritualized behavior.Current Biology, 25(14), 1892-1897.
[29] Mitkidis P., Ayal S., Shalvi S., Heimann K., Levy G., Kyselo M., … Roepstorff A. (2017). The effects of extreme rituals on moral behavior: The performers- observers gap hypothesis.Journal of Economic Psychology, 59, 1-7.
[30] Nielbo, K. L., & Sørensen, J. (2011). Spontaneous processing of functional and non-functional action sequences.Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1(1), 18-30.
[31] Norton, M. I., & Gino, F. (2014). Rituals alleviate grieving for loved ones, lovers, and lotteries.Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 143(1), 266-272.
[32] Park, C. (1999). The influence of the consumption ritualization on buying behaviors.Estud Gerenc, 30(6), 1340-1348.
[33] Piedmont, R. L. (1999). Does spirituality represent the sixth factor of personality? Spiritual transcendence and the five - factor model.Journal of Personality, 67(6), 985-1013.
[34] Ping, A. J., & Peng, K. P. (2018). Gratitude facilitates self-control.Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 16(3), 378-383.
[平安俊, 彭凯平. (2018). 感恩对自我控制的影响.心理与行为研究, 16(3), 378-383.]
[35] Ran Y. X., Wei H. Y., Li Q., & Lei C. (2018). Ritual in the psychology field: A repetitive but meaningful behavior.Advances in Psychological Science, 26(1), 169-179.
[冉雅璇, 卫海英, 李清, 雷超. (2018). 心理学视角下的人类仪式:一种意义深远的重复动作.心理科学进展, 26(1), 169-179.]
[36] Ruiz-Aranda D., Salguero J. M., Cabello R., Palomera R., & Berrocal P. F. (2012). Can an emotional intelligence program improve adolescents' psychosocial adjustment? Results from the INTEMO project.Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 40(8), 1373-1379.
[37] Ruthig J. C., Chipperfield J. G., Perry R. P., Newall N. E., & Swift A. (2007). Comparative risk and perceived control: Implications for psychological and physical well-being among older adults.The Journal of Social Psychology, 147(4), 345-369.
[38] Ruthig J. C., Perry R. P., Hladkyj S., Hall N. C., Pekrun R., & Chipperfield J. G. (2008). Perceived control and emotions: Interactive effects on performance in achievement settings.Social Psychology of Education, 11(2), 161-180.
[39] Sebestény, A., & Emmons, N. (2017). Balinese hindus’ afterlife beliefs as stable constructs: An effect of high frequency domestic rituals.Journal of Cognition and Culture, 17(5), 437-462.
[40] Se, Y. (2012). A religious anthropological analysis about ancestor worship.Journal of Inner Mongolia Normal University (Philosophy & Social Science), 41(3), 29-34.
[色音. (2012). 祖先崇拜的宗教人类学探析. 内蒙古师范大学学报(哲学社会科学版), 41(3), 29-34.]
[41] Tian A. D., Schroeder J., Häubl G., Risen J. L., Norton M. I., & Gino F. (2018). Enacting rituals to improve self-control.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 114(6), 851-876.
[42] Vohs K. D., Wang Y. J., Gino F., & Norton M. I. (2013). Rituals enhance consumption.Psychological Science, 24(9), 1714-1721.
[43] Warburton W. A., Williams K. D., & Cairns D. R. (2006). When ostracism leads to aggression: The moderating effects of control deprivation.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2), 213-220.
[44] Wood A. M., Joseph S., & Linley P. A. (2007). Coping style as a psychological resource of grateful people.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26(9), 1076-1093.
[45] Wu M. L.(2010). Practice of questionnaire statistical analysis (p.244). Chongqing: Chongqing University Press.
[吴明隆. (2010). 问卷统计分析实务 (p.244). 重庆:重庆大学出版社.]
[46] Xiong Y. X.(2015). Religion research on Pumi of China. China Social Sciences Press.
[熊永翔. (2015). 中国普米族宗教研究. 中国社会科学出版社.]
[47] Xu Q., Kwan C. M., & Zhou X. (2020). Helping yourself before helping others: How sense of control promotes charitable behaviors.Journal of Consumer Psychology, 30(3), 486-505.
[48] Yin, K. L., & He, J. M. (2012). Reliability and validity of the mental health continuum short form in adults.Chinese Mental Health Journal, 26(5), 388-392.
[尹可丽, 何嘉梅. (2012). 简版心理健康连续体量表(成人版)的信效度.中国心理卫生杂志, 26(5), 388-392.]
[49] Zeigler-Hill, V. (2006). Contingent self-esteem and the interpersonal circumplex: The interpersonal pursuit of self-esteem.Personality and Individual Differences, 40(4), 713-723.
[50] Zhang, H. Y. (2000). Pumi's "Guolu" and "Zhong Bala". Folklore Studies, (1), 132-135.
[章虹宇. (2000). 普米族的“括鲁”与“仲巴拉”.民俗研究, (1), 132-135.]
[51] Zheng L., Miao M., & Gan Y. Q. (2020). Perceived control buffers the effects of the COVID‐19 pandemic on general health and life satisfaction: The mediating role of psychological distance.Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being, 12(4), 1095-1114.
[52] Zhou X., He L., Yang Q., Lao J., & Baumeister R. F. (2012). Control deprivation and styles of thinking.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 460.
[53] Zou X. Y., Yin K. L., & Lu L. (2018). Collective rituals promote cohesion: Based on action, emotion, and memory.Advances in Psychological Science, 26(5), 939-950.
[邹小燕, 尹可丽, 陆林. (2018). 集体仪式促进凝聚力:基于动作, 情绪与记忆.心理科学进展, 26(5), 939-950.]




[1]蒋元萍, 江程铭, 胡天翊, 孙红月. 绪对跨期决策的影响:来自单维占优模型的解释[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(2): 122-140.
[2]窦凯, 刘耀中, 王玉洁, 聂衍刚. “乐”于合作:感知社会善念诱导合作行为的情绪机制[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(1): 101-114.
[3]龚少英, 上官晨雨, 翟奎虎, 郭雅薇. 情绪设计对多媒体学习的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(6): 771-782.
[4]冯彪;徐亮;张蔚欣;陈婷;王文清;郑希付. 积极情绪对条件性恐惧泛化的抑制作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(3): 317-328.
[5]刘芳; 丁锦红; 张钦. 高、低趋近积极情绪对不同注意加工阶段的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(7): 794-803.
[6]杨沈龙;郭永玉;胡小勇; 舒首立;李静. 低阶层者的系统合理化水平更高吗? ——基于社会认知视角的考察[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(11): 1467-1478.
[7]柳武妹;王海忠;何浏. 人之将尽, 消费国货?死亡信息的暴露增加国货选择的现象、中介和边界条件解析[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(11): 1748-1759.
[8]段锦云;周冉;古晓花. 正面自我标签对建议采纳的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(10): 1591-1602.
[9]王振宏;刘亚;蒋长好. 不同趋近动机强度积极情绪对认知控制的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(5): 546-555.
[10]崔丽霞;张玉静;肖晶;张钦. 积极情绪对心理旋转的影响:趋近动机的调节效应[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(11): 1228-1241.
[11]何晓丽,王振宏,王克静. 积极情绪对人际信任影响的线索效应[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(12): 1408-1417.
[12]王艳梅,郭德俊. 积极情绪对任务转换的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(03): 301-306.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=5130
相关话题/心理 控制 青少年 重庆 宗教