1 西北师范大学心理学院, 兰州 730070
2 Deutsches Resilienz Zentrum, 55131 Mainz, Germany
3 Neuroimaging Center, University Medical Center of the Johannes Gutenberg University, 55131 Mainz, Germany
收稿日期:
2019-01-31出版日期:
2019-12-15发布日期:
2019-10-21通讯作者:
胡传鹏E-mail:hcp4715@hotmail.comNeuroscience bias: Reproducibility and exploration of psychological mechanisms
YIN Jixing1, HU Chuanpeng2,3()1 School of psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou, 730070, China
2 Deutsches Resilienz Zentrum, 55131 Mainz, Germany
3 Neuroimaging Center, University Medical Center of the Johannes Gutenberg University, 55131 Mainz, Germany
Received:
2019-01-31Online:
2019-12-15Published:
2019-10-21Contact:
HU Chuanpeng E-mail:hcp4715@hotmail.com摘要/Abstract
摘要: 神经科学的发展对其他学科以及社会产生了重要的影响。虽然神经科学方法与行为研究方法都是探索人类心理与行为的有效手段并各有所长, 但神经科学的研究结果却可能引起人们过度的解读和信任。研究者发现, 当某一结论使用神经科学结果作为证据时, 比使用行为科学结果或者心理生理学指标作为证据时更加让人信服, 即使神经科学结果与该结论之间毫无关系, 这种现象被称为神经科学偏见(neuroscience bias)。通过系统回顾近年来关于神经科学偏见的研究, 我们发现:(1)虽然神经科学偏见存在可重复性的争论, 但该效应确实存在; (2)神经科学偏见的产生可能是因为个体倾向于还原论的解释(即使用低层次、简单的机制来解释更高层次上的现象)及心理本质主义的影响(即人们认为心理与行为的本质是神经活动)。神经科学偏见反映了公众对科学结果解读的偏见, 未来研究需要探讨这种偏见的心理机制, 从而引导科学结果的正确解读和运用。
图/表 1
表1神经科学偏见文献及其主要结果
原创性研究 | 支持与否 | 重复研究 | 支持与否 |
---|---|---|---|
McCabe & Castel, 2008, Cognition | √ | Rhodes et al., 2014, J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn | √ |
Gurley & Marcus, 2008, Behav Sci Law | √ | Fernandez-Duque et al., 2015, J Cogn Neurosci | √ |
Weisberg et al., 2008, J Cogn Neurosci | √ | Weisberg et al., 2015, Judgm Decis Mak | √ |
McCabe et al., 2011, Behav Sci Law | √ | Schweitzer & Saks, 2011, Behav Sci Law | × |
Keehner et al., 2011, Psychon Bull Rev | √ | Gruber & Dickerson, 2012, Public Underst Sci | × |
Greene & Chill, 2012, Behav Sci Law | √ | Michael et al., 2013, Psychon Bull Rev | × |
Ikeda et al, 2013, Psychon Bull Rev | √ | Schweitzer et al., 2013, Cognition | × |
Munro & Munro, 2014, Basic Appl Soc Psych | √ | West et al., 2014, Basic Appl Soc Psych | × |
Scurich & Shniderman, 2014, PloS one | √ | Appelbaum et al., 2015, Psychol Public Policy Law | × |
Plunkett et al., 2014, Cogsci | √ | Marshall et al, 2017, J Forens Psychiatry Psychol | × |
Shariff & Greene et al, 2014, Psychol Sci | √ | Im, Cho et al, 2018, PloS one | × |
Sapolsky, 2015, Unpublished doctorial dissertation | √ | ||
Diekmann et al., 2015, Int J Sel Assess | √ | ||
Hopkins et al., 2016, Cognition | √ | ||
Minahan & Siedlecki, 2016, Pers Individ Dif | √ | ||
Im et al., 2017, Br J Educ Psychol | √ | ||
Macdonald et al., 2017, Front Psychol | √ | ||
Blakey, 2017, Front Psychol | √ | ||
Schweitzer et al, 2011, Psychol Public Policy Law | × | ||
Baker et al, 2013, PLoS One | × | ||
Hook & Farah, 2013, J Cogn Neurosci | × | ||
Saks et al, 2014, J Empir Leg Stud | × |
表1神经科学偏见文献及其主要结果
原创性研究 | 支持与否 | 重复研究 | 支持与否 |
---|---|---|---|
McCabe & Castel, 2008, Cognition | √ | Rhodes et al., 2014, J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn | √ |
Gurley & Marcus, 2008, Behav Sci Law | √ | Fernandez-Duque et al., 2015, J Cogn Neurosci | √ |
Weisberg et al., 2008, J Cogn Neurosci | √ | Weisberg et al., 2015, Judgm Decis Mak | √ |
McCabe et al., 2011, Behav Sci Law | √ | Schweitzer & Saks, 2011, Behav Sci Law | × |
Keehner et al., 2011, Psychon Bull Rev | √ | Gruber & Dickerson, 2012, Public Underst Sci | × |
Greene & Chill, 2012, Behav Sci Law | √ | Michael et al., 2013, Psychon Bull Rev | × |
Ikeda et al, 2013, Psychon Bull Rev | √ | Schweitzer et al., 2013, Cognition | × |
Munro & Munro, 2014, Basic Appl Soc Psych | √ | West et al., 2014, Basic Appl Soc Psych | × |
Scurich & Shniderman, 2014, PloS one | √ | Appelbaum et al., 2015, Psychol Public Policy Law | × |
Plunkett et al., 2014, Cogsci | √ | Marshall et al, 2017, J Forens Psychiatry Psychol | × |
Shariff & Greene et al, 2014, Psychol Sci | √ | Im, Cho et al, 2018, PloS one | × |
Sapolsky, 2015, Unpublished doctorial dissertation | √ | ||
Diekmann et al., 2015, Int J Sel Assess | √ | ||
Hopkins et al., 2016, Cognition | √ | ||
Minahan & Siedlecki, 2016, Pers Individ Dif | √ | ||
Im et al., 2017, Br J Educ Psychol | √ | ||
Macdonald et al., 2017, Front Psychol | √ | ||
Blakey, 2017, Front Psychol | √ | ||
Schweitzer et al, 2011, Psychol Public Policy Law | × | ||
Baker et al, 2013, PLoS One | × | ||
Hook & Farah, 2013, J Cogn Neurosci | × | ||
Saks et al, 2014, J Empir Leg Stud | × |
参考文献 66
[1] | 胡传鹏, 邓晓红, 周治金, 邓小刚 . ( 2011). 神经法学: 年轻的认知神经科学与古老的法学联姻. 科学通报, 56( 36), 3041-3053. |
[2] | 胡传鹏, 王非, 过继成思, 宋梦迪, 隋洁, 彭凯平 . ( 2016). 心理学研究中的可重复性问题:从危机到契机. 心理科学进展, 24( 9), 1504-1518 |
[3] | 刘媛媛, 丁一, 彭凯平, 胡传鹏 . ( 2019). 多项式加工树模型在社会心理学中的应用. 心理科学, 42( 2), 422-429. |
[4] | Alimardani, A., & Chin, J. M . ( 2019). Neurolaw in Australia: The Use of Neuroscience in Australian Criminal Proceedings. Retrieved January 31, 2019, from . |
[5] | Anderson, P.W . ( 1972). More is different. Science, 177( 4047), 393-396. |
[6] | Appelbaum, P. S., Scurich, N., & Raad, R . ( 2015). Effects of behavioral genetic evidence on perceptions of criminal responsibility and appropriate punishment. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 21( 2), 134-144. |
[7] | Aronson, J. D . ( 2010). The law's use of brain evidence. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 6, 93-108. |
[8] | Baker, D. A., Schweitzer, N., Risko, E. F., & Ware, J. M . ( 2013). Visual attention and the neuroimage bias. PLOS one, 8( 9), e74449. |
[9] | Blakey, R. ( 2017). Does watching a play about the teenage brain affect attitudes toward young offenders? Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 964. |
[10] | Chin, J. M . ( 2014). Psychological science's replicability crisis and what it means for science in the courtroom. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 20( 3), 225-238. |
[11] | Chin, J., Growns, B., & Mellor, D. T. ( 2019. Improving expert evidence: The role of open science and transparency. Retrieved January 31, 2019, from |
[12] | Dar-Nimrod, I., & Heine, S. J . ( 2011). Genetic essentialism: On the deceptive determinism of DNA. Psychological Bulletin, 137( 5), 800-818. |
[13] | Diekmann, J., König, C. J., & Alles, J . ( 2015). The role of neuroscience information in choosing a personality test: Not as seductive as expected. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 23( 2), 99-108. |
[14] | Fernandez-Duque, D., Evans, J., Christian, C., & Hodges, S. D . ( 2015). Superfluous neuroscience information makes explanations of psychological phenomena more appealing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27( 5), 926-944. |
[15] | Funk, C., & Rainie, L. ( 2015). Public and scientists’ views on science and society. Retrieved January 31, 2019, from . |
[16] | Greene, E., & Cahill, B.S . ( 2012). Effects of neuroimaging evidence on mock juror decision making. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 30( 3), 280-296. |
[17] | Gruber, D., & Dickerson, J. A . ( 2012). Persuasive images in popular science: Testing judgments of scientific reasoning and credibility. Public Understanding of Science, 21( 8), 938-948. |
[18] | Gurley, J. R., & Marcus, D. K . ( 2008). The effects of neuroimaging and brain injury on insanity defenses. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 26( 1), 85-97. |
[19] | Hardiman, M., Rinne, L., Gregory, E., & Yarmolinskaya, J . ( 2012). Neuroethics, neuroeducation, and classroom teaching: Where the brain sciences meet pedagogy. Neuroethics, 5( 2), 135-143. |
[20] | Hook, C. J., & Farah, M. J . ( 2013). Look again: Effects of brain images and mind-brain dualism on lay evaluations of research. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25( 9), 1397-1405. |
[21] | Hopkins, E. J., Weisberg, D. S., & Taylor, J. C . ( 2016). The seductive allure is a reductive allure: People prefer scientific explanations that contain logically irrelevant reductive information. Cognition, 155, 67-76. |
[22] | Horikawa, T., Tamaki, M., Miyawaki, Y., & Kamitani, Y . ( 2013). Neural Decoding of Visual Imagery During Sleep. Science, 340( 6132), 639-642. |
[23] | Hu, C.-P., Jiang, X., Jeffrey, R., & Zuo, X.-N . ( 2018). Open science as a better gatekeeper for science and society: A perspective from Neurolaw. Science Bulletin, 63( 23), 1529-1531. |
[24] | Hütter, M., & Klauer, K. C . ( 2016). Applying processing trees in social psychology. European Review of Social Psychology, 27( 1), 116-159. |
[25] | Ikeda, K., Kitagami, S., Takahashi, T., Hattori, Y., & Ito, Y . ( 2013). Neuroscientific information bias in metacomprehension: The effect of brain images on metacomprehension judgment of neuroscience research. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20( 6), 1357-1363. |
[26] | Im, S.-h., Cho, J.-Y., Dubinsky, J. M., & Varma, S . ( 2018). Taking an educational psychology course improves neuroscience literacy but does not reduce belief in neuromyths. PLOS one, 13( 2), e0192163. |
[27] | Im, S. h., Varma, K., & Varma, S . ( 2017). Extending the seductive allure of neuroscience explanations effect to popular articles about educational topics. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87( 4), 518-534. |
[28] | Jacoby, L. L . ( 1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30( 5), 513-541 |
[29] | Janda, L. H., England, K., Lovejoy, D., & Drury, K . ( 1998). Attitudes toward psychology relative to other disciplines. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 29( 2), 140-143. |
[30] | Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow . New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. |
[31] | Keehner, M., Mayberry, L., & Fischer, M. H . ( 2011). Different clues from different views: The role of image format in public perceptions of neuroimaging results. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18( 2), 422-428. |
[32] | Keller, J. (2005). In genes we trust: The biological component of psychological essentialism and its relationship to mechanisms of motivated social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88( 4), 686-702. |
[33] | Kim, N. S., & Keil, F. C . ( 2003). From symptoms to causes: Diversity effects in diagnostic reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 31( 1), 155-165. |
[34] | Krakauer, J. W., Ghazanfar, A. A., Gomez-Marin, A., MacIver, M. A., & Poeppel, D ., ( 2017). Neuroscience needs behavior: Correcting a reductionist bias. Neuron, 93( 3), 480-490. |
[35] | Lilienfeld, S. O . ( 2012). Public skepticism of psychology: Why many people perceive the study of human behavior as unscientific. American Psychologist, 67( 2), 111-129. |
[36] | Lowenberg, K. (2010. fMRI lie detection fails its first hearing on reliability. Retrieved January 31, 2019, from |
[37] | Marshall, J., Lilienfeld, S. O., Mayberg, H., & Clark, S. E . ( 2017). The role of neurological and psychological explanations in legal judgments of psychopathic wrongdoers. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 28( 3), 412-436. |
[38] | McCabe, D. P., & Castel, A. D . ( 2008). Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition, 107( 1), 343-352. |
[39] | McCabe, D. P., Castel, A. D., & Rhodes, M. G . ( 2011). The influence of fMRI lie detection evidence on juror decision- making. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 29( 4), 566-577. |
[40] | Medin, D.L . ( 1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist, 44( 12), 1469-1481. |
[41] | Michael, R. B., Newman, E. J., Vuorre, M., Cumming, G., & Garry, M . ( 2013). On the (non) persuasive power of a brain image. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20( 4), 720-725. |
[42] | Minahan, J., & Siedlecki, K.L . ( 2016). Individual differences in Need for Cognition influence the evaluation of circular scientific explanations. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 113-117. |
[43] | Miton, H., & Mercier, H., (2016). Cognitive obstacles to pro-vaccination beliefs. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19( 11), 633-636. |
[44] | Monterosso, J., Royzman, E. B., & Schwartz, B . ( 2005). Explaining away responsibility: Effects of scientific explanation on perceived culpability. Ethics & Behavior, 15( 2), 139-158. |
[45] | Munro, G.D., & Munro, C. A . ( 2014). “Soft” Versus “Hard” Psychological Science: Biased evaluations of scientific evidence that threatens or supports a strongly held political identity. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36( 6), 533-543. |
[46] | Perrachione, T. K., & Perrachione, J. R . ( 2008). Brains and brands: Developing mutually informative research in neuroscience and marketing. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 7( 4-5), 303-318. |
[47] | Plunkett, D., Lombrozo, T., & Buchak, L . ( 2014). Because the brain agrees: The impact of neuroscientific explanations for belief. Cognitive Science, 36(36), 1180-1185. |
[48] | Redmond, E. C., & Griffith, C. J . ( 2004). Consumer perceptions of food safety risk, control and responsibility. Appetite, 43( 3), 309-313. |
[49] | Rhodes, R. E., Rodriguez, F., & Shah, P . ( 2014). Explaining the alluring influence of neuroscience information on scientific reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40( 5), 1432-1440. |
[50] | Roets, A., & van H., A . ( 2011). The role of need for closure in essentialist entitativity beliefs and prejudice: An epistemic needs approach to racial categorization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50( 1), 52-73. |
[51] | Saks, M. J., Schweitzer, N., Aharoni, E., & Kiehl, K. A . ( 2014). The impact of neuroimages in the sentencing phase of capital trials. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 11( 1), 105-131. |
[52] | Sapolsky, Z. G . ( 2015). Neuroscience information's effect on causal explanations of psychological disorders and treatment recommendations (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Long Island University, New York, U.S. |
[53] | Schauer, F. (2010). Neuroscience, lie-detection, and the law: Contrary to the prevailing view, the suitability of brain- based lie-detection for courtroom or forensic use should be determined according to legal and not scientific standards. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14( 3), 101-103. |
[54] | Schweitzer, N., Baker, D. A., & Risko, E. F . ( 2013). Fooled by the brain: Re-examining the influence of neuroimages. Cognition, 129( 3), 501-511. |
[55] | Schweitzer, N. J., & Saks, M. J . ( 2011). Neuroimage evidence and the insanity defense. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 29( 4), 592-607. |
[56] | Schweitzer, N. J., Saks, M. J., Murphy, E. R., Roskies, A. L., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & Gaudet, L. M . ( 2011). Neuroimages as evidence in a mens rea defense: No impact. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 17( 3), 357-393. |
[57] | Scurich, N., & Shniderman, A. (2014). The selective allure of neuroscientific explanations. PLOS one, 9( 9), e107529. |
[58] | Shariff, A. F., Greene, J. D., Karremans, J. C., Luguri, J. B., Clark, C. J., Schooler, J. W., .. Vohs, K. D . ( 2014). Free will and punishment: A mechanistic view of human nature reduces retribution. Psychological Science, 25( 8), 1563-1570. |
[59] | Spranger, T. M . (Ed.) ( 2011). International Neurolaw: A Comparative Analysis. Berlin, Germany: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. |
[60] | Szucs, D., & Ioannidis, J. P . ( 2017). Empirical assessment of published effect sizes and power in the recent cognitive neuroscience and psychology literature. Plos Biology, 15( 3), e2000797. |
[61] | Turnwald, B. P., Goyer, J. P., Boles, D. Z., Silder, A., Delp, S. L., & Crum, A. J . ( 2018). Learning one’s genetic risk changes physiology independent of actual genetic risk. Nature Human Behaviour. 3, 48-56. |
[62] | Webster, M. (1973). Psychological reductionism, methodological individualism, and large-scale problems. American Sociological Review, 38( 2), 258-273. |
[63] | Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. R . ( 2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20( 3), 470-477. |
[64] | Weisberg, D. S., Taylor, J. C., & Hopkins, E. J . ( 2015). Deconstructing the seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Judgment and Decision Making, 10 (5), 429-441. |
[65] | West, M. L., Lawson, V. Z., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2014). The effect of electrophysiological neuroscientific deception detection evidence on juror judgments in a criminal trial. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36 (2), 133-144. |
[66] | Yzerbyt, V., Rocher, S. & Schadron, G.. ( 1997). Stereotypes as explanations: A subjective essentialistic view of group perception. In R. Spears, P. J. Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of stereotyping and group life (pp. 20-50). Malden, Massachusetts,, USA: Blackwell Publishing. |
相关文章 15
[1] | 衡书鹏, 赵换方, 范翠英, 周宗奎. 视频游戏虚拟化身对自我概念的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(5): 810-823. |
[2] | 衡书鹏, 赵换方, 孙丽君, 周宗奎. 虚拟销售代理的拟人效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(5): 884-904. |
[3] | 刘传军, 廖江群. 具身效应何处寻:解决可重复性危机的分析性途径[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(12): 2260-2271. |
[4] | 赵娜, 马敏, 辛自强. 生命意义感获取的心理机制及其影响因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(6): 1003-1011. |
[5] | 余小霞;苑媛;辛自强. 文字与数字量尺的差异及心理机制:兼论量尺制作的方法学问题[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(2): 201-210. |
[6] | 王财玉;雷雳. 网络购物情境下的羊群效应:内涵、影响因素与机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(2): 298-311. |
[7] | 胡传鹏;王非;过继成思;宋梦迪; 隋洁;彭凯平. 心理学研究中的可重复性问题:从危机到契机[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(9): 1504-1518. |
[8] | 张琪;尹天子;冉光明. 动态面孔表情优势效应的心理机制及神经基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(9): 1514-1522. |
[9] | 刘程浩;徐富明;王伟;李燕;史燕伟. 概率判断中的合取谬误[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(6): 967-978. |
[10] | 秦秋霞;于海涛; 乔亲才. 全球化时代跨界民族国家认同的心理机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(5): 745-754. |
[11] | 李爱梅;高结怡;彭元;夏萤;陈晓曦. 积极情感和消极情感适应的不对称性及其机制探讨[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(4): 632-642. |
[12] | 李英武;于宙;韩笑;刘婷安胥. 一般心理能力(GMA)预测绩效:现状、机制及趋势[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(3): 448-459. |
[13] | 段锦云;张倩. 建言行为的认知影响因素、理论基础及发生机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2012, 20(1): 115-126. |
[14] | 汪芬;黄宇霞. 正念的心理和脑机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(11): 1635-1644. |
[15] | 蒋多; 徐富明; 陈雪玲; 刘腾飞; 张军伟. 资本市场中投资者羊群行为的心理机制及其影响因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(5): 810-818. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4881