1广东第二师范学院教师教育学院, 广州 510303
2华南师范大学心理学院
3华南师范大学心理应用研究中心, 广州 510631
4广东省心理健康与认知科学重点实验室, 广州 510631
5东莞市东莞中学, 东莞 523120
6西安外国语大学学生心理健康教育中心, 西安 710128
收稿日期:
2020-02-27发布日期:
2021-04-25通讯作者:
郑希付E-mail:zhengxifu@m.scnu.edu.cn基金资助:
国家自然科学基金项目(32000752);国家自然科学基金项目(31771218);国家自然科学基金项目(31970996);教育部人文社会科学研究项目(20YJC190009);广东省哲学社会科学规划项目(GD19YXL01);幸福广州心理服务与辅导基地, 广东省普通高校哲学社会科学重点实验室(2020WSYS002)Effects of prediction error and acute stress on retrieval-extinction of fear memories of different strengths
LI Junjiao1, CHEN Wei2,3,4, HU Yanjian5, CAOYANG Jingwen6, ZHENG Xifu2,3,4()1College of Teacher Education, Guangdong University of Education, Guangzhou 510303, China
2School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
3Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China
4Guangdong Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science, Guangzhou 510631, China
5Dongguan Middle School, Dongguan 523120, China
6Student Mental Health Education Center, Xi'an International Studies University, Xi'an 710128, China
Received:
2020-02-27Published:
2021-04-25Contact:
ZHENG Xifu E-mail:zhengxifu@m.scnu.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 在条件性恐惧记忆再巩固模型下, 预期错误被证明是引发记忆不稳定的必要条件, 但其在不同强度恐惧记忆下的作用尚不明确。对于高强度可能导致的提取无效, 缺乏相应的探索以寻找解决办法, 而应激(stress)在其中发挥的作用值得探索。本研究考察人类被试中, 预期错误在不同强度恐惧记忆下的作用, 以及提取之后施加外源性应激对于消退进程的影响。结果发现, 对于较弱的恐惧记忆, 单个预期错误提取后消退可显著抑制恐惧自发恢复; 而对于较强恐惧记忆, 单个预期错误不能提取恐惧记忆进入再巩固, 已消退的记忆还会复发; 且在该种情况下, 如果在提取后施加外源性急性应激, 会进一步增大恐惧恢复。
图/表 8
表1实验分组和样本量
变量 | 组别 | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
G1 | G2 | G3 | ||
提取阶段 | 单个PE | 单个PE+SECPT | 单个PE | |
N (SCR) | 16 | 19 | 15 | |
N (FPS) | 15 | 21 | 23 | |
年龄(岁) | 20.88 ± 1.54 | 20.04 ± 1.80 | 21.44 ± 2.58 | 0.069 |
STAI-T | 37.35 ± 10.88 | 41.83 ± 7.10 | 39.78 ± 7.92 | 0.266 |
BDI | 6.94 ± 4.53 | 8.44 ± 5.96 | 7.26 ± 5.83 | 0.655 |
表1实验分组和样本量
变量 | 组别 | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
G1 | G2 | G3 | ||
提取阶段 | 单个PE | 单个PE+SECPT | 单个PE | |
N (SCR) | 16 | 19 | 15 | |
N (FPS) | 15 | 21 | 23 | |
年龄(岁) | 20.88 ± 1.54 | 20.04 ± 1.80 | 21.44 ± 2.58 | 0.069 |
STAI-T | 37.35 ± 10.88 | 41.83 ± 7.10 | 39.78 ± 7.92 | 0.266 |
BDI | 6.94 ± 4.53 | 8.44 ± 5.96 | 7.26 ± 5.83 | 0.655 |
图2第一天、第二天各生理指标与问卷测量的时间点 注:BDI:贝克抑郁量表; STAI-T:特质焦虑问卷; PANAS:正性负性情绪量表; BP:血压; HR:心率; SF:主观感受; 各名称之后的数字代表测试的时间序号。
图2第一天、第二天各生理指标与问卷测量的时间点 注:BDI:贝克抑郁量表; STAI-T:特质焦虑问卷; PANAS:正性负性情绪量表; BP:血压; HR:心率; SF:主观感受; 各名称之后的数字代表测试的时间序号。
图1实验程序示意图。A:总的实验流程图; B:习得阶段恐惧强度操作及提取阶段示意图。 注:对恐惧强度的操纵为:在第一天使用CS呈现后半段不可预期时间点出现的两次电击(US), 形成CS-不可预知的US联合, 预期产生较高强度恐惧记忆(G1、G2); 使用在CS呈现时间的最后固定时间点(分别出现在第4800 ms和第5800 ms) 出现的两个US, 形成CS-可预知的US联合, 预期产生较低强度的恐惧记忆(G3)。对预期错误(PE)的操作为:在第二天对于所有组使用最后一个US (第5800 ms)进行提取, 在所有组造成电击次数上的预期错误, 即原本被试在习得阶段都是进行两次电击, 形成了对于每个CS都跟随两次US的预期。但在提取阶段CS呈现时, 只跟随1次US, 因此会形成单个预期错误(single PE)。
图1实验程序示意图。A:总的实验流程图; B:习得阶段恐惧强度操作及提取阶段示意图。 注:对恐惧强度的操纵为:在第一天使用CS呈现后半段不可预期时间点出现的两次电击(US), 形成CS-不可预知的US联合, 预期产生较高强度恐惧记忆(G1、G2); 使用在CS呈现时间的最后固定时间点(分别出现在第4800 ms和第5800 ms) 出现的两个US, 形成CS-可预知的US联合, 预期产生较低强度的恐惧记忆(G3)。对预期错误(PE)的操作为:在第二天对于所有组使用最后一个US (第5800 ms)进行提取, 在所有组造成电击次数上的预期错误, 即原本被试在习得阶段都是进行两次电击, 形成了对于每个CS都跟随两次US的预期。但在提取阶段CS呈现时, 只跟随1次US, 因此会形成单个预期错误(single PE)。
表2各组在社会评价冰水应激/温水任务中心率变异性频率域参数值对比
实验分组 | G1 不可预期US_无应激 n= 15 | G2 不可预期US_应激 n= 20 | G3 可预期US_无应激 n= 22 | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
LF | |||||
前(pre) | 71.70 ± 40.66 | 128.85 ± 108.95 | 120.87 ± 126.45 | 1.46 | 0.24 |
中(mid) | 106.56 ± 145.84 | 227.76 ± 313.37 | 84.53 ± 82.59 | 2.29 | 0.11 |
后(post) | 125.57 ± 109.65 | 130.52 ± 168.65 | 149.50 ± 181.47 | 0.12 | 0.89 |
HF | |||||
前(pre) | 44.83 ± 38.65 | 96.02 ± 86.74 | 68.66 ± 65.67 | 2.43 | 0.10 |
中(mid) | 73.57 ± 78.74 | 102.51 ± 100.09 | 74.11 ± 55.91 | 0.51 | 0.61 |
后(post) | 55.46 ± 28.64 | 63.04 ± 40.96 | 88.76 ± 64.18 | 0.12 | 0.89 |
表2各组在社会评价冰水应激/温水任务中心率变异性频率域参数值对比
实验分组 | G1 不可预期US_无应激 n= 15 | G2 不可预期US_应激 n= 20 | G3 可预期US_无应激 n= 22 | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
LF | |||||
前(pre) | 71.70 ± 40.66 | 128.85 ± 108.95 | 120.87 ± 126.45 | 1.46 | 0.24 |
中(mid) | 106.56 ± 145.84 | 227.76 ± 313.37 | 84.53 ± 82.59 | 2.29 | 0.11 |
后(post) | 125.57 ± 109.65 | 130.52 ± 168.65 | 149.50 ± 181.47 | 0.12 | 0.89 |
HF | |||||
前(pre) | 44.83 ± 38.65 | 96.02 ± 86.74 | 68.66 ± 65.67 | 2.43 | 0.10 |
中(mid) | 73.57 ± 78.74 | 102.51 ± 100.09 | 74.11 ± 55.91 | 0.51 | 0.61 |
后(post) | 55.46 ± 28.64 | 63.04 ± 40.96 | 88.76 ± 64.18 | 0.12 | 0.89 |
图3SECPT任务过程中各组在心率变异性低频成分上的分值比较
图3SECPT任务过程中各组在心率变异性低频成分上的分值比较
图4三组被试在恐惧记忆习得、提取、消退和测试阶段的皮肤电反应 注:横坐标代表试次:1~6为第一天习得阶段, 8为第二天提取试次, 10~19为第二天消退阶段, 21~28为第三天自发恢复测试与消退阶段, 30~37为第三天重建测试与消退阶段。误差线表示标准误, 黄色闪电符号代表电击。图6同。
图4三组被试在恐惧记忆习得、提取、消退和测试阶段的皮肤电反应 注:横坐标代表试次:1~6为第一天习得阶段, 8为第二天提取试次, 10~19为第二天消退阶段, 21~28为第三天自发恢复测试与消退阶段, 30~37为第三天重建测试与消退阶段。误差线表示标准误, 黄色闪电符号代表电击。图6同。
图5各组在恐惧自发恢复量和重建量上的对比。A以皮肤电(SCR)为测量指标的恐惧恢复对比; B以惊跳反应(FPS)为测量指标的恐惧恢复对比。 注:恐惧自发恢复量 = 再消退的第一个CS+试次的恐惧反应 - 消退训练最后一个CS+的恐惧反应; 恐惧重建量 = 最终消退的第一个CS+试次的恐惧反应 - 再消退最后一个CS+的恐惧反应。*p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01。
图5各组在恐惧自发恢复量和重建量上的对比。A以皮肤电(SCR)为测量指标的恐惧恢复对比; B以惊跳反应(FPS)为测量指标的恐惧恢复对比。 注:恐惧自发恢复量 = 再消退的第一个CS+试次的恐惧反应 - 消退训练最后一个CS+的恐惧反应; 恐惧重建量 = 最终消退的第一个CS+试次的恐惧反应 - 再消退最后一个CS+的恐惧反应。*p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01。
图6三组被试在恐惧记忆习得、提取、消退和测试阶段的惊跳反应
图6三组被试在恐惧记忆习得、提取、消退和测试阶段的惊跳反应
参考文献 70
[1] | Abrari, K., Rashidy-Pour, A., Semnanian, S., & Fathollahi, Y. (2008). Administration of corticosterone after memory reactivation disrupts subsequent retrieval of a contextual conditioned fear memory: Dependence upon training intensity. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 89(2),178-184. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2007.07.005. URLpmid: 17702613 |
[2] | Agren, T., Bjorkstrand, J., & Fredrikson, M. (2017). Disruption of human fear reconsolidation using imaginal and in vivo extinction. Behavioural Brain Research, 319,9-15. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.11.014. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.11.014URL |
[3] | Akirav, I., & Maroun, M. (2013). Stress modulation of reconsolidation. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 226(4),747-761. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1007/s00213-012-2887-6. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1007/s00213-012-2887-6URL |
[4] | Alberini, C.M., Milekic, M.H., & Tronel, S. (2006). Mechanisms of memory stabilization and de-stabilization. Cellular & Molecular Life Sciences, 63(9),999-1008. doi: 10.1007/s00018-006-6025-7. |
[5] | Amadi, U., Lim, S.H., Liu, E., Baratta, M.V., & Goosens, K.A. (2017). Hippocampal processing of ambiguity enhances fear memory. Psychological Science, 28(2),143-161. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1177/0956797616674055. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1177/0956797616674055URL |
[6] | Blumenthal, T.D., Cuthbert, B.N., Filion, D.L., Hackley, S., Lipp, O.V.,& van Boxtel, A. (2005). Committee report: Guidelines for human startle eyeblink electromyographic studies. Psychophysiology, 42(1),1-15. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00271.x. URLpmid: 15720576 |
[7] | Bos, M.G.N., van Goethem, T.H.J., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2014). Cortisol response mediates the effect of post-reactivation stress exposure on contextualization of emotional memories. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50,72-84. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.030. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.030URL |
[8] | Brugnera, A., Zarbo, C., Adorni, R., Tasca, G.A., Rabboni, M., Bondi, E.… Sakatani, K. (2017). Cortical and cardiovascular responses to acute stressors and their relations with psychological distress. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 114,38-46. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.02.002URL |
[9] | Cai, W.H., Blundell, J., Han, J., Greene, R.W., & Powell, C.M. (2006). Postreactivation glucocorticoids impair recall of established fear memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(37),9560-9566. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2397-06.2006. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2397-06.2006URL |
[10] | Castaldo, R., Melillo, P., Bracale, U., Caserta, M., Triassi, M., & Pecchia, L. (2015). Acute mental stress assessment via short term hrv analysis in healthy adults: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Biomedical Signal Processing and Control, 18,370-377. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.bspc.2015.02.012. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.bspc.2015.02.012URL |
[11] | Chen, W., Li, J., Caoyang, J., Yang, Y., Hu, Y., & Zheng, X. (2018). Effects of prediction error on post-retrieval extinction of fear to compound stimuli. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(7),739-749. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.3724/sp.J.1041.2018.00739. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00739URL |
[ 陈伟, 李俊娇, 曹杨婧文, 杨勇, 胡琰健, 郑希付. (2018). 预期错误在复合恐惧记忆提取消退中的作用. 心理学报>, 50( 7),739-749. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.3724/sp.J.1041.2018.00739. ] | |
[12] | Das, R.K., Lawn, W., & Kamboj, S.K. (2015). Rewriting the valuation and salience of alcohol-related stimuli via memory reconsolidation. Translational Psychiatry, 5,e645. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/tp.2015.132. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/tp.2015.132URL |
[13] | Davis, M. (2006). Neural systems involved in fear and anxiety measured with fear-potentiated startle. American Psychologist, 61(8),741-756. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.741URL |
[14] | Diaz-Mataix, L., Ruiz Martinez, R.C., Schafe, G.E., LeDoux, J.E., & Doyere, V. (2013). Detection of a temporal error triggers reconsolidation of amygdala-dependent memories. Current Biology, 23(6),467-472. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.053. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.053URL |
[15] | Drexler, S.M., Merz, C.J., Hamacher-Dang, T.C., Tegenthoff, M., & Wolf, O.T. (2015). Effects of cortisol on reconsolidation of reactivated fear memories. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(13),3036-3043. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/npp.2015.160. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/npp.2015.160URLpmid: 26058664 |
[16] | Duvarci, S., & Nader, K. (2004). Characterization of fear memory reconsolidation. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(42),9269-9275. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2971-04.2004. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2971-04.2004URL |
[17] | Eisenberg, M., Kobilo, T., Berman, D.E., & Dudai, Y. (2003). Stability of retrieved memory: Inverse correlation with trace dominance. Science, 301(5636),1102-1104. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1126/science.1086881. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1126/science.1086881URL |
[18] | Elsey, J.W.B., & Kindt, M. (2017). Tackling maladaptive memories through reconsolidation: From neural to clinical science. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 142(Pt A),108-117. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2017.03.007. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2017.03.007URL |
[19] | Fernandez, R.S., Boccia, M.M., & Pedreira, M.E. (2016). The fate of memory: Reconsolidation and the case of prediction error. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 68,423-441. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.004. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.004URL |
[20] | Gera, R., Barak, S., & Schonberg, T. (2019). Counterconditioning following memory retrieval diminishes the reinstatement of appetitive memories in humans. Scientific Reports, 9(1),9213. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/s41598-019-45492-6. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/s41598-019-45492-6URL |
[21] | Golkar, A., Tjaden, C., & Kindt, M. (2017). Vicarious extinction learning during reconsolidation neutralizes fear memory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 92,87-93. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.brat.2017.02.004. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.brat.2017.02.004URL |
[22] | Goltseker, K., Bolotin, L., & Barak, S. (2017). Counterconditioning during reconsolidation prevents relapse of cocaine memories. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(3),716-726. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/npp.2016.140. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/npp.2016.140URLpmid: 27468918 |
[23] | Hamm, A.O., Cuthbert, B.N., Globisch, J., & Vaitl, D. (1997). Fear and the startle reflex: Blink modulation and autonomic response patterns in animal and mutilation fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 34(1),97-107. doi: doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02420.x. URLpmid: 9009813 |
[24] | Hu, J., Wang, W., Homan, P., Wang, P., Zheng, X., & Schiller, D. (2018). Reminder duration determines threat memory modification in humans. Scientific Reports, 8(1),8848. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/s41598-018-27252-0. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/s41598-018-27252-0URL |
[25] | Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., Hardt, O., & Nadel, L. (2007). Reconsolidation of episodic memories: A subtle reminder triggers integration of new information. Learning and Memory, 14(1-2),47-53. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1101/lm.365707. doi: 10.1101/lm.421807URL |
[26] | James, E.L., Bonsall, M.B., Hoppitt, L., Tunbridge, E.M., Geddes, J.R., Milton, A.L., & Holmes, E.A. (2015). Computer game play reduces intrusive memories of experimental trauma via reconsolidation-update mechanisms. Psychological Science, 26(8),1201-1215. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1177/0956797615583071. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1177/0956797615583071URLpmid: 26133572 |
[27] | Kindt, M. (2018). The surprising subtleties of changing fear memory: A challenge for translational science. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 373(1742). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0033. |
[28] | Kindt, M., & Soeter, M. (2013). Reconsolidation in a human fear conditioning study: A test of extinction as updating mechanism. Biological Psychology, 92(1),43-50. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.016. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.016URL |
[29] | Kindt, M., Soeter, M., & Vervliet, B. (2009). Beyond extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 12(3),256-258. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/nn.2271. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/nn.2271URL |
[30] | Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., & Cuthbert, B.N. (1998). Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biological Psychiatry, 44(12),1248-1263. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00275-3URL |
[31] | Lee, J.L.C., Milton, A.L., & Everitt, B.J. (2006). Reconsolidation and extinction of conditioned fear: Inhibition and potentiation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(39),10051-10056. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1523/jneurosci.2466-06.2006. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2466-06.2006URL |
[32] | Li, J., Chen, W., Caoyang, J., Hu, Y., Yang, Y., Xu, L. … Zheng, X. (2019). Role of prediction error in destabilizing fear memories in retrieval extinction and its neural mechanisms. Cortex, 121,292-307. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.09.003. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.09.003URL |
[33] | Li, J., Chen, W., Caoyang, J., Wu, W., Jie, J., Xu, L., & Zheng, X. (2017). Moderate partially reduplicated conditioned stimuli as retrieval cue can increase effect on preventing relapse of fear to compound stimuli. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11,575. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00575. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00575URL |
[34] | Liu, J., Zhao, L., Xue, Y., Shi, J., Suo, L., Luo, Y.… Lu, L. (2014). An unconditioned stimulus retrieval extinction procedure to prevent the return of fear memory. Biological Psychiatry, 76(11),895-901. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.027. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.027URL |
[35] | Lovibond, P.F., & Shanks, D.R. (2002). The role of awareness in pavlovian conditioning: Empirical evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 28(1),3-26. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1037/0097-7403.28.1.3. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1037/0097-7403.28.1.3URL |
[36] | Luo, Y.X., Xue, Y.X., Liu, J.F., Shi, H.S., Jian, M., Han, Y.… Lu, L., (2015). A novel ucs memory retrieval- extinction procedure to inhibit relapse to drug seeking. Nature Communications, 6,7675. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/ncomms8675. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/ncomms8675URL |
[37] | Meir Drexler, S., & Wolf, O.T. (2017). Stress disrupts the reconsolidation of fear memories in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 77,95-104. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.027. doi: S0306-4530(16)30558-3URLpmid: 28024275 |
[38] | Milad, M.R., Rauch, S.L., Pitman, R.K., & Quirk, G.J. (2006). Fear extinction in rats: Implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biological Psychology, 73(1),61-71. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.008. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.008URL |
[39] | Monfils, M.-H., Cowansage, K.K., Klann, E., & LeDoux, J.E. (2009). Extinction-reconsolidation boundaries: Key to persistent attenuation of fear memories. Science, 324(951),951-955. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1167975. doi: 10.1126/science.1167975URL |
[40] | Nader, K., Schafe, G.E.& Le Doux, J.E. (2000). Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature, 406,722-726. pmid: 10963596 |
[41] | Phelps, E.A., & Hofmann, S.G. (2019). Memory editing from science fiction to clinical practice. Nature, 572,43-50. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/s41586-019-1433-7. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/s41586-019-1433-7URL |
[42] | Raio, C.M., Brignoni-Perez, E., Goldman, R., & Phelps, E.A. (2014). Acute stress impairs the retrieval of extinction memory in humans. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 112,212-221. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2014.01.015. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2014.01.015URL |
[43] | Raio, C.M., Orederu, T.A., Palazzolo, L., Shurick, A.A., & Phelps, E.A. (2013). Cognitive emotion regulation fails the stress test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(37),15139-15144. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305706110. |
[44] | Rescorla, R.A., & Wagner, A.R. (1972). A theory of pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Current Research and Theory,64-99. |
[45] | Robinson, M.J.F., & Franklin, K.B.J. (2010). Reconsolidation of a morphine place preference: Impact of the strength and age of memory on disruption by propranolol and midazolam. Behavioural Brain Research, 213(2),201-207. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.056. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.056URLpmid: 20457186 |
[46] | Robinson, O.J., Overstreet, C., Charney, D.R., Vytal, K., & Grillon, C. (2013). Stress increases aversive prediction error signal in the ventral striatum. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(10),4129-4133. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1073/pnas.1213923110. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1073/pnas.1213923110URLpmid: 23401511 |
[47] | Schiller, D., Monfils, M.H., Raio, C.M., Johnson, D.C., Ledoux, J.E., & Phelps, E.A. (2010). Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature, 463(7277),49-53. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/nature08637. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/nature08637URLpmid: 20010606 |
[48] | Schubert, C., Lambertz, M., Nelesen, R.A., Bardwell, W., Choi, J.B., & Dimsdale, J.E. (2009). Effects of stress on heart rate complexity—A comparison between short-term and chronic stress. Biological Psychology, 80(3),325-332. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.11.005pmid: 19100813 |
[49] | Schwabe, L., & Schachinger, H. (2018). Ten years of research with the socially evaluated cold pressor test: Data from the past and guidelines for the future. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 92,155-161. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.03.010. doi: S0306-4530(17)31608-6URLpmid: 29573884 |
[50] | Schwabe, L., & Wolf, O.T. (2010). Socially evaluated cold pressor stress after instrumental learning favors habits over goal-directed action. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(7),977-986. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.12.010. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.12.010URLpmid: 20071096 |
[51] | Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2012). Retrieval per se is not sufficient to trigger reconsolidation of human fear memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 97(3),338-345. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.01.009. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.01.009URLpmid: 22406658 |
[52] | Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2013). Prediction error governs pharmacologically induced amnesia for learned fear. Science, 339,830-833. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1126/science.1231357. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1126/science.1231357URL |
[53] | Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2014a). Fear conditioning of scr but not the startle reflex requires conscious discrimination of threat and safety. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8,32. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00032. |
[54] | Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2014b). Prediction error demarcates the transition from retrieval, to reconsolidation, to new learning. Learning and Memory, 21(11),580-584. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1101/lm.035493.114. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1101/lm.035493.114URL |
[55] | Sevenster, D., Visser, R.M., & D'Hooge, R. (2018). A translational perspective on neural circuits of fear extinction: Current promises and challenges. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 155,113-126. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2018.07.002. doi: S1074-7427(18)30153-9URLpmid: 29981423 |
[56] | Sinclair, A.H., & Barense, M.D. (2018). Surprise and destabilize: Prediction error influences episodic memory reconsolidation. Learning and Memory, 25(8),369-381. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1101/lm.046912.117. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1101/lm.046912.117URL |
[57] | Sinclair, A.H., & Barense, M.D. (2019). Prediction error and memory reactivation: How incomplete reminders drive reconsolidation. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(10),727-739. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.tins.2019.08.007. doi: S0166-2236(19)30151-1URLpmid: 31506189 |
[58] | Soeter, M., & Kindt, M. (2010). Dissociating response systems: Erasing fear from memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 94(1),30-41. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.03.004. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.03.004URL |
[59] | Soeter, M., & Kindt, M. (2011). Disrupting reconsolidation: Pharmacological and behavioral manipulations. Learning and Memory, 18(6),357-366. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1101/lm.2148511. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1101/lm.2148511URL |
[60] | Soeter, M., & Kindt, M. (2012a). Erasing fear for an imagined threat event. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(11),1769-1779. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.03.011. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.03.011URL |
[61] | Soeter, M., & Kindt, M. (2012b). Stimulation of the noradrenergic system during memory formation impairs extinction learning but not the disruption of reconsolidation. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(5),1204-1215. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/npp.2011.307. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/npp.2011.307URL |
[62] | Suzuki, A., Josselyn, S.A., Frankland, P.W., Masushige, S., Silva, A.J., & Kida, S. (2004). Memory reconsolidation and extinction have distinct temporal and biochemical signatures. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(20),4787-4795. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5491-03.2004. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5491-03.2004URL |
[63] | Tronel, S., & Alberini, C.M. (2007). Persistent disruption of a traumatic memory by post-retrieval inactivation of glucocorticoid receptors in the amygdala. Biological Psychiatry, 62(1),33-39. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.009URL |
[64] | Vervliet, B., Craske, M.G., & Hermans, D. (2013). Fear extinction and relapse: State of the art. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9,215-248. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542URLpmid: 23537484 |
[65] | Wagner, J., Lampert, R., Tennen, H., & Feinn, R. (2015). Exposure to discrimination and heart rate variability reactivity to acute stress among women with diabetes. Stress and Health, 31(3),255-262. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1002/smi.2542. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1002/smi.2542URLpmid: 24194397 |
[66] | Wang, S.H., de Oliveira Alvares, L., & Nader, K. (2009). Cellular and systems mechanisms of memory strength as a constraint on auditory fear reconsolidation. Nature Neuroscience, 12(7),905-912. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/nn.2350. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1038/nn.2350URL |
[67] | Weike, A.I., Schupp, H.T., & Hamm, A.O. (2007). Fear acquisition requires awareness in trace but not delay conditioning. Psychophysiology, 44(1),170-180. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00469.x. |
[68] | Xue, Y.X., Luo, Y.X., Wu, P., Shi, H.S., Xue, L.F., Chen, C.… Lu, L. (2012). A memory retrieval-extinction procedure to prevent drug craving and relapse. Science, 336(6078),241-245. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1126/science.1215070. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1126/science.1215070URL |
[69] | Yang, Y., Jie, J., Li, J., Chen, W., & Zheng, X. (2019). A novel method to trigger the reconsolidation of fear memory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 122,103461. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.brat.2019.103461. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.brat.2019.103461URL |
[70] | Zuccolo, P.F., & Hunziker, M.H.L. (2019). A review of boundary conditions and variables involved in the prevention of return of fear after post-retrieval extinction. Behavioural Processes, 162,39-54. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.beproc.2019.01.011. doi: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/10.1016/j.beproc.2019.01.011URL |
相关文章 10
[1] | 王红波, 邢小莉, 王慧颖. 普萘洛尔修复即刻消退产生的二次创伤[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(6): 603-612. |
[2] | 胡娜,陈安涛,王宴庆,李晴,许珍珍,龙泉杉. 急性应激损伤错误监控与错误后调整[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(2): 162-172. |
[3] | 罗禹,念靖晴,鲍未,张静静,赵守盈,潘运,许爽,张禹. 急性应激损害对威胁刺激的注意解除[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 26-37. |
[4] | 胡静初,张蔚欣,陈小婷,王文清,王子洁,庄楚群,冯彪,郑希付. 远期恐惧记忆再巩固更新机制的线索选择性特点[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(3): 316-323. |
[5] | 陈伟,李俊娇,曹杨婧文,杨勇,胡琰健,郑希付. 预期错误在复合恐惧记忆提取消退中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(7): 739-749. |
[6] | 廖素群;郑希付. 认知重评对负性效价的抑制促进条件性恐惧消退[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(4): 352-361. |
[7] | 徐亮;区诵宜;郑希付;陈婷;冯彪;闫沛. 状态焦虑对条件性恐惧泛化的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(12): 1507-1518. |
[8] | 孙楠,魏艺铭,李倩,郑希付. 条件性恐惧记忆消退返回的性别差异[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(3): 314-321. |
[9] | 李培培,张丽丽,韦美,李敏. 条件性恐惧大鼠边缘下区Cdk5激酶活性、caspase-3表达以及突触结构的变化[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(05): 544-552. |
[10] | 李欢欢,林文娟,李俊发. 急性情绪应激对大鼠行为和脑神经颗粒素磷酸化水平的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2006, 38(04): 576-582. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4955