1武汉理工大学马克思主义学院, 武汉 430070
2北京师范大学发展心理研究院, 北京 100875
3武汉大学哲学学院, 武汉 430072
收稿日期:
2019-08-16出版日期:
2020-11-25发布日期:
2020-09-22通讯作者:
杨春亮E-mail:chunliang.yang@bnu.edu.cn基金资助:
* 中央高校基本科研业务费专项资金资助(2020VI022);中央高校基本科研业务费专项资金资助(2019NTSS28)Forward testing effect on new learning in older adults
WANG Tangsheng1,3, YANG Chunliang2(), ZHONG Nian31School of Marxism, Wuhan University of Technology, Wuhan 430070, China
2Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
3School of Philosophy, Wuhan University, Wuhan 430072, China
Received:
2019-08-16Online:
2020-11-25Published:
2020-09-22Contact:
YANG Chunliang E-mail:chunliang.yang@bnu.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 为检验前置测试是否可以有效地提升老年人学习新事物的能力,参与实验的老年人被随机分为有前置测试的实验组和无前置测试的对照组,在实验1中学习了5列单词,在实验2中记忆了5列日用品,在实验3中学习了4段讲座视频。结果表明,实验组老年人对新事物的记忆成绩均高于对照组的老年人。结论认为,前置测试能够显著提升老年人学习新事物的能力,干扰降低与学习参与相结合的“加减”理论能更好地解释提升老年人记忆的前向测试效应。
图/表 7
图1前向测试效应的实验流程 注:实验组(又称测试组)的每一列事物学习之后即进行当列测试, 对照组(又称演算组或重学组)在前几列事物学习之后进行演算或者重新学习等其他任务。实验组和对照组最后一列学完后均进行当列测试, 然后进行总体测试。
图1前向测试效应的实验流程 注:实验组(又称测试组)的每一列事物学习之后即进行当列测试, 对照组(又称演算组或重学组)在前几列事物学习之后进行演算或者重新学习等其他任务。实验组和对照组最后一列学完后均进行当列测试, 然后进行总体测试。
表1实验1实验组和对照组被试在控制变量上的组间差异
控制变量 | 实验组(n = 15) | 对照组(n = 15) | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 66.73 ± 5.73 | 66.13 ± 3.92 | 0.33 | 0.74 |
教育程度(年限) | 15.13 ± 3.27 | 15.92 ± 4.98 | 0.63 | 0.53 |
健康(5级) | 3.53 ± 0.74 | 3.33 ± 0.98 | 0.30 | 0.61 |
表1实验1实验组和对照组被试在控制变量上的组间差异
控制变量 | 实验组(n = 15) | 对照组(n = 15) | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 66.73 ± 5.73 | 66.13 ± 3.92 | 0.33 | 0.74 |
教育程度(年限) | 15.13 ± 3.27 | 15.92 ± 4.98 | 0.63 | 0.53 |
健康(5级) | 3.53 ± 0.74 | 3.33 ± 0.98 | 0.30 | 0.61 |
图2A: 临时测试在5列中的回忆成绩B: 5列临时测试中的前摄干扰 C: 最终测试中回忆起的每列的单词量(实验1) 注:误差条代表± 1个标准误
图2A: 临时测试在5列中的回忆成绩B: 5列临时测试中的前摄干扰 C: 最终测试中回忆起的每列的单词量(实验1) 注:误差条代表± 1个标准误
表2实验2实验组和对照组被试在控制变量上的组间差异
控制变量 | 实验组(n = 16) | 对照组(n = 16) | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 66.47 ± 4.93 | 65.63 ± 4.24 | 0.51 | 0.61 |
教育程度(年限) | 9.93 ±3.28 | 9.25 ± 2.29 | 0.68 | 0.51 |
健康(5级) | 2.73 ± 0.46 | 2.75 ± 0.77 | -0.07 | 0.94 |
词汇得分(总分40) | 36.87 ± 3.31 | 35.94 ± 3.15 | 0.80 | 0.43 |
工作记忆 | 5.73 ± 0.98 | 5.40 ± 0.95 | 0.94 | 0.35 |
认知加工精度(%) | 92.96 ± 7.99 | 89.58 ± 10.32 | 1.02 | 0.32 |
认知加工速度(秒) | 6.54 ± 1.11 | 6.29 ± 1.81 | 0.46 | 0.65 |
表2实验2实验组和对照组被试在控制变量上的组间差异
控制变量 | 实验组(n = 16) | 对照组(n = 16) | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 66.47 ± 4.93 | 65.63 ± 4.24 | 0.51 | 0.61 |
教育程度(年限) | 9.93 ±3.28 | 9.25 ± 2.29 | 0.68 | 0.51 |
健康(5级) | 2.73 ± 0.46 | 2.75 ± 0.77 | -0.07 | 0.94 |
词汇得分(总分40) | 36.87 ± 3.31 | 35.94 ± 3.15 | 0.80 | 0.43 |
工作记忆 | 5.73 ± 0.98 | 5.40 ± 0.95 | 0.94 | 0.35 |
认知加工精度(%) | 92.96 ± 7.99 | 89.58 ± 10.32 | 1.02 | 0.32 |
认知加工速度(秒) | 6.54 ± 1.11 | 6.29 ± 1.81 | 0.46 | 0.65 |
图3A: 临时测试在5列中的回忆成绩 B: 5列临时测试中的前摄干扰 C: 最终测试中回忆起的每列的物品量(实验2) 注:误差条代表 ± 1个标准误
图3A: 临时测试在5列中的回忆成绩 B: 5列临时测试中的前摄干扰 C: 最终测试中回忆起的每列的物品量(实验2) 注:误差条代表 ± 1个标准误
表3实验3实验组和对照组被试在控制变量上的组间差异
控制变量 | 实验组(n = 25) | 对照组(n = 24) | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 67.16 ± 6.44 | 66.96 ± 6.20 | 0.11 | 0.91 |
教育程度(年限) | 10.92 ± 3.11 | 11.04 ± 3.11 | -0.14 | 0.89 |
健康(5级) | 3.08 ± 0.64 | 3.21 ± 0.83 | -0.61 | 0.55 |
词汇得分(总分40) | 37.08 ± 3.21 | 36.21 ± 4.23 | 0.81 | 0.42 |
工作记忆 | 5.84 ± 1.07 | 5.73 ± 1.22 | 0.34 | 0.74 |
认知加工精度(%) | 96.22 ± 5.25 | 96.30 ± 4.54 | -0.05 | 0.96 |
认知加工速度(秒) | 6.37 ± 2.13 | 5.73 ± 1.22 | 0.48 | 0.64 |
表3实验3实验组和对照组被试在控制变量上的组间差异
控制变量 | 实验组(n = 25) | 对照组(n = 24) | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 67.16 ± 6.44 | 66.96 ± 6.20 | 0.11 | 0.91 |
教育程度(年限) | 10.92 ± 3.11 | 11.04 ± 3.11 | -0.14 | 0.89 |
健康(5级) | 3.08 ± 0.64 | 3.21 ± 0.83 | -0.61 | 0.55 |
词汇得分(总分40) | 37.08 ± 3.21 | 36.21 ± 4.23 | 0.81 | 0.42 |
工作记忆 | 5.84 ± 1.07 | 5.73 ± 1.22 | 0.34 | 0.74 |
认知加工精度(%) | 96.22 ± 5.25 | 96.30 ± 4.54 | -0.05 | 0.96 |
认知加工速度(秒) | 6.37 ± 2.13 | 5.73 ± 1.22 | 0.48 | 0.64 |
图4A:第4段视频临时测试成绩. B: 最终测试成绩(实验3) 注:误差条代表 ± 1 个标准误.
图4A:第4段视频临时测试成绩. B: 最终测试成绩(实验3) 注:误差条代表 ± 1 个标准误.
参考文献 41
[1] | Aslan, A., & B?uml, K.-H. T.(2015). Testing enhances subsequent learning in older but not in younger elementary school children. Developmental Science, 19(6), 992-998. doi: 10.1111/desc.12340URLpmid: 26614638 |
[2] | Chan, J., Meissner, C. A., & Davis, S. D.(2018). Retrieval potentiates new learning: A theoretical and meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(11), 1111-1146. URLpmid: 30265011 |
[3] | Cho, K. W., Neely, J. H., Crocco, S., & Vitrano, D.(2017). Testing enhances both encoding and retrieval for both tested and untested items. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(7), 1211-1235. |
[4] | Craik, F. I. M., & Rose, N. S.(2012). Memory encoding and aging: A neurocognitive perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(7), 1729-1739. URLpmid: 22155274 |
[5] | Farrimond, S. J., Knight, R. G., & Titov, N.(2003). Prospective remembering and aging. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(7), 769-769. |
[6] | Gluck, J., & Bluck, S.(2007). Looking back across the life span: A life story account of the reminiscence bump. Memory and Cognition, 35(8), 1928-1939. doi: 10.3758/bf03192926URLpmid: 18265609 |
[7] | Gong, Y. X.(1992). Revised Wechsler adult intelligence scale manual in China. Hunan Map Press. |
[ 龚耀先 (1992). 中国修订韦氏成人智力量表手册. 湖南地图出版社] | |
[8] | GréGoire, J., & van der Linden, M.(1997). Effect of age on forward and backward digit spans. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 4(2), 140-149. |
[9] | Hafkemeijer, A., van der Grond, J., & Rombouts, S. A. R. B..,(2012). Imaging the default mode network in aging and dementia. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta, 1822(3), 431-441. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.07.008URLpmid: 21807094 |
[10] | Ikier, S., & Hasher, L.(2006). Age differences in implicit interference. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61(5), 278-284. |
[11] | Ikier, S., Yang, L. X., & Hasher, L.(2008). Implicit proactive interference, age, and automatic versus controlled retrieval strategies. Psychological Science, 19(5), 456-461. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02109.xURLpmid: 18466406 |
[12] | Jackson, J. D., & Balota, D. A.(2012). Mind-wandering in younger and older adults: Converging evidence from the sustained attention to response task and reading for comprehension. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 106-119. URLpmid: 21707183 |
[13] | Jing, H. G., Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L.(2016). Interpolated testing influences focused attention and improves integration of information during a video-recorded lecture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22(3), 305-318. URLpmid: 27295464 |
[14] | Jordano, M. L., & Touron, D. R.(2017). Stereotype threat as a trigger of mind-wandering in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 32(3), 307-313. URLpmid: 28333503 |
[15] | Kielar, A., Shah-Basak, P. P., Deschamps, T., Jokel, R., & Meltzer, J. A.(2019). Slowing is slowing: Delayed neural responses to words are linked to abnormally slow resting state activity in primary progressive aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 126(6), 331-347. |
[16] | Krawietz S. A., Tamplin, A. K., & Radvansky, G. A.(2012). Aging and mind wandering during text comprehension. Psychology and Aging, 27(4), 951-958. URLpmid: 22686406 |
[17] | Levy, B., & Langer, E.(1994). Aging free from negative stereotypes: Successful memory in china among the american deaf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(6), 989-997. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.66.6.989URLpmid: 8046582 |
[18] | Nunes, L. D., & Weinstein, Y(2012). Testing improves true recall and protects against the build-up of proactive interference without increasing false recall. Memory, 20(2), 138-154. URLpmid: 22292715 |
[19] | Old, S. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M(2008). Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 23(1), 104-118. URLpmid: 18361660 |
[20] | Past?tter, B., & B?uml, K.-H. T.(2014). Retrieval practice enhances new learning: The forward effect of testing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(4), 1-5. |
[21] | Past?tter, B., & B?uml, K.-H. T.(2019). Testing enhances subsequent learning in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 34(2), 242-250. doi: 10.1037/pag0000307URLpmid: 30335442 |
[22] | Past?tter, B., Schicker, S., Niedernhuber, J., & B?uml, K.-H. T.(2011). Retrieval during learning facilitates subsequent memory encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(2), 287-297. |
[23] | Past?tter, B., Weber, J., & B?uml, K.-H. T.(2013). Using testing to improve learning after severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 27(2), 280-285. URLpmid: 23527656 |
[24] | Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Benali, K., & Eustache, F.(2002). Episodic and semantic remote autobiographical memory in ageing. Memory, 10(4), 239-257. URLpmid: 12097209 |
[25] | Schacter, D. L., & Szpunar, K. K.(2015). Enhancing attention and memory during video-recorded lectures. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 1(1), 60-71. |
[26] | Seli, P., Maillet, D., Smilek, D., Oakman, J. M., & Schacter, D. L.(2017). Cognitive aging and the distinction between intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychology and Aging, 32(4), 315-324. URLpmid: 28471215 |
[27] | Szpunar, K. K., Jing, H. G., & Schacter, D. L.(2014). Overcoming overconfidence in learning from video-recorded lectures: Implications of interpolated testing for online education. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3(3), 161-164. |
[28] | Szpunar, K. K., Khan, N. Y., & Schacter, D. L.(2013). Interpolated memory tests reduce mind wandering and improve learning of online lectures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(16), 6313-6317. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221764110URLpmid: 23576743 |
[29] | Szpunar, K. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L.(2008). Testing during study insulates against the buildup of proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(6), 1392-1399. |
[30] | Wang, D. H., Shen, J. L., Peng, H. M., Tang, D., & Zhang, L., The model of educational effect on old adult's cognition.Acta Psychologica Sinica, 37(4), 511-516. |
[ 王大华, 申继亮, 彭华茂, 唐丹, 张凌.(2005). 教育水平对老年人认知能力的影响模式. 心理学报, 37(4), 511-516] | |
[31] | Wechsler, D.(1981). Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale. Oxford, England: Psychological Corp. |
[32] | Weinstein, Y., Gilmore, A. W., Szpunar, K. K., & McDermott, K. B.(2014). The role of test expectancy in the build-up of proactive interference in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(4), 1039-1048. |
[33] | Weinstein, Y., McDermott, K. B., & Szpunar, K. K.(2011). Testing protects against proactive interference in face-name learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18(3), 518-523. |
[34] | Whitebourne, S. K., & Whitebourne, S. B.(2010). Adult development and aging: Biopsychosocial perspectives (5th ed.). USA: John Willey & Sons. |
[35] | Wiggs, C. L., Weisberg, J., & Martin, A.(2006). Repetition priming across the adult lifespan - The long and short of it. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 13(3-4), 308-325. |
[36] | Wissman, K. T., Rawson K. A., & Pyc, M. A.(2011). The interim test effect: Testing prior material can facilitate the learning of new material. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18(6), 1140-1147. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0140-7URLpmid: 21796496 |
[37] | Yang, C. L., Chew, S. J., Sun, B. K., & Shanks, D. R.(2019). The forward effects of testing transfer to different domains of learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(5), 809-826. |
[38] | Yang, C. L., Potts, R., & Shanks, D. R.(2017). The forward testing effect on self-regulated study time allocation and metamemory monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 23(3), 263-277. URLpmid: 28333482 |
[39] | Yang, C. L., Potts, R., & Shanks, D. R.(2018). Enhancing learning and retrieval of new information: A review of the forward testing effect. npj Scientific of Learning, 3(8), 1-9. |
[40] | Yang, C. L., & Shanks, D. R.(2018). The forward testing effect: Interim testing enhances inductive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 44(3), 485-492. |
[41] | Zavagnin, M., Borella, E., & de Beni, R.(2014). When the mind wanders: Age-related differences between young and older adults. Acta Psychologica, 145, 54-64. |
相关文章 11
[1] | 吴国婷, 张敏强, 倪雨菡, 杨亚威, 漆成明, 吴健星. 老年人孤独感及其影响因素的潜在转变分析[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(9): 1061-1070. |
[2] | 彭华茂,毛晓飞. 抑制对老年人舌尖现象的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(10): 1142-1150. |
[3] | 姚若松,郭梦诗,叶浩生. 社会支持对老年人社会幸福感的影响机制:希望与孤独感的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(10): 1151-1158. |
[4] | 张奇林;周艺梦. 中国城镇低龄退休老年人工作与幸福感的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(4): 472-481. |
[5] | 周爱保;刘沛汝;张彦驰;尹玉龙. 老年人的朋友参照效应[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(9): 1143-1151. |
[6] | 王大华;杨小洋;王岩;Richard B. Miller. 老年人夫妻依恋的测量及与一般依恋的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(9): 1133-1142. |
[7] | 陈栩茜;张积家;朱云霞. 言语产生老化中的抑制损伤:来自不同任务的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(3): 329-343. |
[8] | 肖红蕊;龚先旻;王大华. 老年人错误记忆的情绪效价和时间效应[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(7): 922-930. |
[9] | 任娜,佐斌,侯飞翔,汪国驹. 情境效应或自动化加工:大学生对老年人的内隐态度[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(6): 777-788. |
[10] | 王大华,佟雁,周丽清,申继亮. 亲子支持对老年人主观幸福感的影响机制[J]. 心理学报, 2004, 36(01): 78-82. |
[11] | 张卫东. 老年人特殊生活事件的应对意向与情境性应对[J]. 心理学报, 1998, 30(04): 401-408. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4823