1对外经济贸易大学国际商学院, 北京 100029
2清华大学心理学系, 北京 100084
3加州大学圣迭戈分校雷迪管理学院, 拉霍亚 92093 美国
4清华大学深圳研究生院, 深圳 518055
收稿日期:
2019-07-10出版日期:
2020-10-25发布日期:
2020-08-24通讯作者:
李虹E-mail:lhong@mail.tsinghua.edu.cn基金资助:
* 国家自然科学基金项目(71871124);国家社科基金重点项目(20AZD085)The relationship between anticipated communication and creativity: Moderating role of construal level
LUAN Mo1,2, WU Shuang2,3, LI Hong2,4()1International School of Business, University of International Business and Economics, Beijing 100029, China
2Department of Psychology, Tsinghua University, Beijing 100084, China
3Rady School of Management, University of California-San Diego, La Jolla, CA, 92093, USA
4Tsinghua Shenzhen International Graduate School at Shenzhen, Tsinghua University, Shenzhen 518055, China
Received:
2019-07-10Online:
2020-10-25Published:
2020-08-24Contact:
LI Hong E-mail:lhong@mail.tsinghua.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 该研究探讨了预期交流对创造力的影响, 以及解释水平在其中的调节作用。实验1采用结构性想象任务探讨预期交流是否影响创造力。结果表明, 相较于没有预期交流, 预期交流条件下的个体表现出更高的创造力。实验2采取了创意产生任务, 并探讨了解释水平在其中的调节作用。结果表明, 当完成抽象的、高解释水平任务时, 相较于无预期交流, 预期交流条件下个体在新奇性和变通性两个维度上表现出了更高的创造力; 当完成具体的、低解释水平任务时, 预期交流的效应不复存在。也就是说, 只有当创造力任务要求高解释水平的抽象思维时, 预期交流才能促进创造力的发挥。总结而言, 本研究在过往对于交流与创造力以及解释水平与创造力的研究基础上进一步发现, 对于抽象创造力任务, 虽然真正的信息交流尚未发生, 但仅仅对于交流的预期就会提高创造力水平。
图/表 5
图1研究的理论框架
图1研究的理论框架
表1不同预期交流条件下的新奇性得分、新奇特征数、熟悉度、享受度和动机
预期交流 | 新奇性得分 | 新奇特征数 | 熟悉度 | 享受度 | 动机 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
有(N = 53) | 4.59 | 2.13 | 2.04 | 1.35 | 1.47 | 0.95 | 5.05 | 1.30 | 5.38 | 1.02 |
无(N = 57) | 3.31 | 1.87 | 1.26 | 1.21 | 1.77 | 1.10 | 5.13 | 1.46 | 5.37 | 1.03 |
表1不同预期交流条件下的新奇性得分、新奇特征数、熟悉度、享受度和动机
预期交流 | 新奇性得分 | 新奇特征数 | 熟悉度 | 享受度 | 动机 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
有(N = 53) | 4.59 | 2.13 | 2.04 | 1.35 | 1.47 | 0.95 | 5.05 | 1.30 | 5.38 | 1.02 |
无(N = 57) | 3.31 | 1.87 | 1.26 | 1.21 | 1.77 | 1.10 | 5.13 | 1.46 | 5.37 | 1.03 |
图2不同交流预期和任务解释水平下的新奇性得分(误差线表示95%置信区间, **p < 0.01)
图2不同交流预期和任务解释水平下的新奇性得分(误差线表示95%置信区间, **p < 0.01)
图3不同交流预期和任务解释水平下的变通性得分(误差线表示95%置信区间, **p < 0.01)
图3不同交流预期和任务解释水平下的变通性得分(误差线表示95%置信区间, **p < 0.01)
图4不同交流预期和任务解释水平下的流畅性得分(误差线表示95%置信区间)
图4不同交流预期和任务解释水平下的流畅性得分(误差线表示95%置信区间)
参考文献 44
[1] | Aiken, M., & Hage, J. (1971). The organic organization and innovation. Sociology, 5(1), 63-82. |
[2] | Amabile, T. M. (1979). Effects of external evaluation on artistic creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(2), 221-233. |
[3] | Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(5), 997-1013. |
[4] | Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(2), 357-376. |
[5] | Amabile, T. M. (1985). Motivation and creativity: Effects of motivational orientation on creative writers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(2), 393-399. |
[6] | Amabile, T. M., & Pillemer, J. (2012). Perspectives on the social psychology of creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 46(1), 3-15. |
[7] | Ancona, D. G., & Caldwell, D. F. (1992). Demography and design: Predictors of new product team performance. Organization Science, 3(3), 321-341. |
[8] | Barron, F. (1955). The disposition toward originality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(3), 478-485. |
[9] | Dijksterhuis, A., & Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: The generative power of unconscious thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(1), 135-146. |
[10] | Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2003). Effects of communication goals and expectancies on language abstraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 682-696. URLpmid: 12703643 |
[11] | Ebadi, Y. M., & Utterback, J. M. (1984). The effects of communication on technological innovation. Management Science, 30(5), 572-585. |
[12] | F?rster, J., Friedman, R. S., & Liberman, N. (2004). Temporal construal effects on abstract and concrete thinking: Consequences for insight and creative cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2), 177-189. URLpmid: 15301626 |
[13] | Grant, A. M., & Berry, J. (2011). The necessity of others is the mother of invention: Intrinsic and prosocial motivations, perspective taking, and creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 54( 1), 73-96. |
[14] | Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Publications. |
[15] | Hirt, E. R., Devers, E. E., & McCrea, S. M. (2008). I want to be creative: Exploring the role of hedonic contingency theory in the positive mood-cognitive flexibility link. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 214-230. URLpmid: 18211173 |
[16] | Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1122-1131. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.6.1122URLpmid: 3598858 |
[17] | Jia, L., Hirt, E. R., & Karpen, S. C. (2009). Lessons from a faraway land: The effect of spatial distance on creative cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1127-1131. |
[18] | Joshi, P. D., & Wakslak, C. J. (2014). Communicating with the crowd: Speakers use abstract messages when addressing larger audiences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 351-362. doi: 10.1037/a0032413URL |
[19] | Joshi, P. D., Wakslak, C. J., Raj, M., & Trope, Y. (2016). Communicating with distant others: The functional use of abstraction. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(1), 37-44. |
[20] | Kanter, R. M. (1988). Three tiers for innovation research. Communication Research, 15(5), 509-523. |
[21] | Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. New York, NY: Macmillan. |
[22] | Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the "overjustification" hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28(1), 129-137. |
[23] | Lu, J. G., Akinola, M., & Mason, M. F. (2017). “Switching On” creativity: Task switching can increase creativity by reducing cognitive fixation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 139, 63-75. |
[24] | Lu, J. G., Hafenbrack, A. C., Eastwick, P. W., Wang, D. J., Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2017). “Going out” of the box: Close intercultural friendships and romantic relationships spark creativity, workplace innovation, and entrepreneurship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(7), 1091-1108. doi: 10.1037/apl0000212URLpmid: 28358527 |
[25] | Monge, P. R., Cozzens, M. D., & Contractor, N. S. (1992). Communication and motivational predictors of the dynamics of organizational innovation. Organization Science, 3(2), 250-274. |
[26] | Mumford, M. D., & Gustafson, S. B. (1988). Creativity syndrome: Integration, application, and innovation. Psychological Bulletin, 103(1), 27-43. |
[27] | Paulus, P. B., & Nijstad, B. A. (2003). Group creativity: Innovation through collaboration Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press Innovation through collaboration. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. |
[28] | Plucker, J. A., & Makel, M. C. (2010). Assessment of creativity. In J. C.Kaufman & R. J.Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 48-73). Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. |
[29] | Polman, E., & Emich, K. J. (2011). Decisions for others are more creative than decisions for the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(4), 492-501. doi: 10.1177/0146167211398362URLpmid: 21317316 |
[30] | Sawyer, R. K. (2006). Educating for innovation. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 1(1), 41-48. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2005.08.001URL |
[31] | Semin, G. R., de Montes, G. L., & Valencia, J. F. (2003). Communication constraints on the linguistic intergroup bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(2), 142-148. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00523-1URL |
[32] | Shalley, C. E., & Gilson, L. L. (2004). What leaders need to know: A review of social and contextual factors that can foster or hinder creativity. The Leadership Quarterly, 15(1), 33-53. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2003.12.004URL |
[33] | Shalley, C. E., & Perry-Smith, J. E. (2001). Effects of social-psychological factors on creative performance: The role of informational and controlling expected evaluation and modeling experience. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 84(1), 1-22. URLpmid: 11162295 |
[34] | Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., Cram, J. T., Hess, K. I., ... Richard, C. A. (2008). Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks: Exploring the reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2(2), 68-85. doi: 10.1037/1931-3896.2.2.68URL |
[35] | Simonton, D. K. (2000). Creativity: Cognitive, personal, developmental, and social aspects. American Psychologist, 55(1), 151-158. URLpmid: 11392859 |
[36] | Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(6), 845-851. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.845URLpmid: 16393019 |
[37] | Steinmetz, J., & Pfattheicher, S. (2017). Beyond social facilitation: A review of the far-reaching effects of social attention. Social Cognition, 35(5), 585-599. |
[38] | Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440-463. doi: 10.1037/a0018963URLpmid: 20438233 |
[39] | Uziel, L. (2007). Individual differences in the social facilitation effect: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(3), 579-601. |
[40] | Uziel, L. (2010). Look at me, I’m happy and creative: The effect of impression management on behavior in social presence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(12), 1591-1602. doi: 10.1177/0146167210386239URLpmid: 20956356 |
[41] | Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 660-671. |
[42] | van de Ven, A. H. (1986). Central problems in the management of innovation. Management Science, 32(5), 590-607. |
[43] | Ward, T. B. (1994). Structured imagination: The role of category structure in exemplar generation. Cognitive Psychology, 27(1), 1-40. |
[44] | Woodman, R. W., Sawyer, J. E., & Griffin, R. W. (1993). Toward a theory of organizational creativity. Academy of Management Review, 18(2), 293-321. |
相关文章 15
[1] | 张景焕, 付萌萌, 辛于雯, 陈佩佩, 沙莎. 小学高年级学生创造力的发展:性别差异及学校支持的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(9): 1057-1070. |
[2] | 胡巧婷,王海江,龙立荣. 新员工工作重塑会带来积极的结果吗?领导成员交换与个体传统性的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(5): 659-668. |
[3] | 朱金强, 徐世勇, 周金毅, 张柏楠, 许昉昉, 宗博强. 跨界行为对创造力影响的跨层次双刃剑效应[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(11): 1340-1351. |
[4] | 罗萍,施俊琦,朱燕妮,房俨然. 个性化工作协议对员工主动性职业行为和创造力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 81-92. |
[5] | 李婉悦,韩尚锋,刘燊,杨亚平,张林,徐强. 场景对面孔情绪探测的影响:特质性焦虑的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(8): 869-878. |
[6] | 卫利华,刘智强,廖书迪,龙立荣,廖建桥. 集体心理所有权、地位晋升标准与团队创造力[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(6): 677-687. |
[7] | 沈伊默,马晨露,白新文,诸彦含,鲁云林,张庆林,刘军. 辱虐管理与员工创造力:心理契约破坏和中庸思维的不同作用[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(2): 238-247. |
[8] | 常淑敏,张丽娅,王玲晓. 发展资源在减少青少年外化问题行为中的累积效应及关系模式[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(11): 1244-1255. |
[9] | 刘伟国, 房俨然, 施俊琦, 莫申江. 领导创造力期望对团队创造力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(6): 667-677. |
[10] | 张勇, 刘海全, 王明旋, 青 平. 挑战性压力和阻断性压力对员工创造力的影响:自我效能的中介效应与组织公平的调节效应[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(4): 450-461. |
[11] | 任志洪, 张雅文, 江光荣. 正念冥想对焦虑症状的干预: 效果及其影响因素元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(3): 283-305. |
[12] | 朱瑜, 吕阳, 王雁飞, 王丽璇. 教练型领导如何影响员工创新? 跨层次被调节的中介效应[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(3): 327-336. |
[13] | 刘圣明,陈力凡,王思迈. 满招损, 谦受益:团队沟通视角下谦卑型领导行为对团队创造力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(10): 1159-1168. |
[14] | 杨林川, 马红宇, 姜海, 梁娟, 齐玲. 社会公正对权威合法性的影响: 社会阶层的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(7): 980-994. |
[15] | 杨颖;朱毅. 谁该成为体验型产品网络评论的主角, 图片还是文字?[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(8): 1026-1036. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4806