
1 福建师范大学心理学院, 福州 350117
2 三明学院教育与音乐学院, 福建 三明 365004
收稿日期:
2019-05-17出版日期:
2020-06-25发布日期:
2020-04-22通讯作者:
孟迎芳E-mail:175695016@qq.com基金资助:
* 国家自然科学基金青年项目(31800906);2018年福建省自然科学基金面上项目(2018J01719)Effects of target detection on memory retrieval
HUANG Yanqing1,2, MENG Yingfang1(
1 School of Psychology, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou 350117, China
2 School of Education and Music, Sanming University, Sanming 365004, China
Received:
2019-05-17Online:
2020-06-25Published:
2020-04-22Contact:
MENG Yingfang E-mail:175695016@qq.com摘要/Abstract
摘要: 采用经典的“学习-再认”范式, 在测验阶段设置了目标探测的干扰任务, 通过比较目标探测和分心拒绝是否会对记忆提取产生不同的影响, 以进一步探讨提取干扰与再认记忆之间的关系。结果发现, 无论对目标进行显性(实验1)还是隐性(实验2)的探测反应, 目标探测下旧词的再认反应均要比分心拒绝下更优, 表明外显记忆的提取也会受到注意资源的调节。但更为重要的是, 在辨别力指数(d′)及判断标准(C)上的差异分析发现, 目标探测并没有提高被试的再认水平, 而是降低了被试的判断标准, 使得被试在目标探测条件下更容易对词做出“旧”反应。这种反应倾向与目标探测的动作反应方式无关, 也不受词汇的编码加工水平深浅的调节。由此可见, 外显记忆提取并非完全的“自动化加工”, 仍会受到注意资源的调节。
图/表 3
表1按键反应下的再认反应时(ms)和正确率
指标 | 深加工旧词 | 浅加工旧词 | 新词 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | 目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | 目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | |
反应时 | 798 (206) | 929 (233) | 824 (246) | 891 (255) | 987 (315) | 892 (233) |
正确率 | 0.81 (0.14) | 0.61 (0.20) | 0.57 (0.18) | 0.34 (0.19) | 0.60 (0.21) | 0.77 (0.19) |
表1按键反应下的再认反应时(ms)和正确率
指标 | 深加工旧词 | 浅加工旧词 | 新词 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | 目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | 目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | |
反应时 | 798 (206) | 929 (233) | 824 (246) | 891 (255) | 987 (315) | 892 (233) |
正确率 | 0.81 (0.14) | 0.61 (0.20) | 0.57 (0.18) | 0.34 (0.19) | 0.60 (0.21) | 0.77 (0.19) |
表2实验1和实验2各类旧词的辨别力d′及判断标准C
条件类型 | 实验1 | 实验2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
d′ | C | d′ | C | |
深加工目标词 | 1.32 (0.82) | -0.32 (0.44) | 1.51 (0.72) | -0.38 (0.46) |
深加工分心词 | 1.23 (0.92) | 0.30 (0.45) | 1.36 (0.76) | 0.31 (0.52) |
浅加工目标词 | 0.55 (0.35) | 0.07 (0.57) | 1.50 (0.39) | 0.13 (0.58) |
浅加工分心词 | 0.48 (0.56) | 0.68 (0.57) | 1.37 (0.54) | 0.81 (0.47) |
表2实验1和实验2各类旧词的辨别力d′及判断标准C
条件类型 | 实验1 | 实验2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
d′ | C | d′ | C | |
深加工目标词 | 1.32 (0.82) | -0.32 (0.44) | 1.51 (0.72) | -0.38 (0.46) |
深加工分心词 | 1.23 (0.92) | 0.30 (0.45) | 1.36 (0.76) | 0.31 (0.52) |
浅加工目标词 | 0.55 (0.35) | 0.07 (0.57) | 1.50 (0.39) | 0.13 (0.58) |
浅加工分心词 | 0.48 (0.56) | 0.68 (0.57) | 1.37 (0.54) | 0.81 (0.47) |
表3计数条件下的再认反应时(ms)和正确率
指标 | 深加工旧词 | 浅加工旧词 | 新词 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | 目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | 目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | |
反应时 | 736 (188) | 814 (219) | 785 (235) | 887 (280) | 871 (210) | 808 (257) |
正确率 | 0.85 (0.11) | 0.61 (0.21) | 0.54 (0.19) | 0.28 (0.14) | 0.62 (0.22) | 0.80 (0.13) |
表3计数条件下的再认反应时(ms)和正确率
指标 | 深加工旧词 | 浅加工旧词 | 新词 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | 目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | 目标探测 | 分心拒绝 | |
反应时 | 736 (188) | 814 (219) | 785 (235) | 887 (280) | 871 (210) | 808 (257) |
正确率 | 0.85 (0.11) | 0.61 (0.21) | 0.54 (0.19) | 0.28 (0.14) | 0.62 (0.22) | 0.80 (0.13) |
参考文献 43
[1] | Anderson, N. D., Craik, F. I. M., & Naveh-Benjamin, M . (1998). The attentional demands of encoding and retrieval in younger and older adults: Ⅰ. Evidence from divided attention costs. Psychology and Aging, 13(3), 405-423. |
[2] | Anderson, N. D., Iidaka, T., Cabeza, R., Kapur, S., McIntosh, A. R., & Craik, F. I. M . (2000). The effects of divided attention on encoding-and retrieval-related brain activity: A pet study of younger and older adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 775-792. doi: 10.1162/089892900562598URL |
[3] | Aston-Jones, G. D., & Cohen, J . (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 403-450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709URL |
[4] | Baddeley, A., Lewis, V., Eldridge, M., & Thomson, N . (1984). Attention and retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(4), 518-540. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.113.4.518URL |
[5] | Cohen, J . (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159. |
[6] | Craik, F. I. M., Eftekhari, E., & Binns, M. A . (2018). Effects of divided attention at encoding and retrieval: Further data. Memory & Cognition, 46(8), 1263-1277. |
[7] | Craik, F. I. M., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Anderson, N. D . (1996). The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(2), 159-180. |
[8] | Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S . (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671-684. |
[9] | Craik, F. I. M., Naveh-Benjamin, M., Ishaik, G., & Anderson, N. D . (2000). Divided attention during encoding and retrieval: Differential control effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(6), 1744-1749. |
[10] | Fernandes, M. A., & Moscovitch, M . (2000). Divided attention and memory: Evidence of substantial interference effects at retrieval and encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(2), 155-176. |
[11] | Fernandes, M. A., & Moscovitch, M . (2003). Interference effects from divided attention during retrieval in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 18(2), 219-230. |
[12] | Hicks, J. L., & Marsh, R. L . (2000). Toward specifying the attentional demands of recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(6), 1483-1498. |
[13] | Iidaka, T., Anderson, N. D., Kapur, S., Cabez, R., & Craik, F. I. M . (2000). The effect of divided attention on encoding and retrieval in episodic memory revealed by positron emission tomography. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(2), 267-280. doi: 10.1162/089892900562093URL |
[14] | Jacoby, L. L . (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory & Language, 30(5), 513-541. |
[15] | Jacoby, L. L., & Whitehouse, K . (1989). An illusion of memory: False recognition influenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(2), 126-135. |
[16] | Kaneko, Y., & Sakai, K . (2015). Dissociation in decision bias mechanism between probabilistic information and previous decision. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 261. |
[17] | Kent, C., & Lamberts, K . (2006). The time course of perception and retrieval in matching and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(4), 920-931. |
[18] | Kurilla, B. P., & Westerman, D. L . (2008). Processing fluency affects subjective claims of recollection. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 82-92. |
[19] | Lozito, J. P., & Mulligan, N. W . (2006). Exploring the role of attention during memory retrieval: Effects of semantic encoding and divided attention. Memory & Cognition, 34(5), 986-998. |
[20] | Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D . (2004). Detection theory: A user's guide (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology press. |
[21] | Makovski, T., Swallow, K. M., & Jiang, Y. V . (2011). Attending to unrelated targets boosts short-term memory for color arrays. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1498-1505. |
[22] | Meng, Y. F . (2010). The asymmetric relationship between encoding and retrieval. Advances in Psychological Science, 18(12), 1926-1933. |
[ 孟迎芳 . (2010). 记忆编码与提取的非对称关系. 心理科学进展, 18(12), 1926-1933.] | |
[23] | Meng, Y. F., & Guo, C. Y . (2007). The asymmetric effect of interference at encoding or retrieval on implicit and explicit memory. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39(4), 579-588. |
[ 孟迎芳, 郭春彦 . (2007). 编码与提取干扰对内隐和外显记忆的非对称性影响. 心理学报, 39(4), 579-588.] | |
[24] | Meng, Y. F., & Guo, C. Y . (2009). The asymmetric relationship between encoding and retrieval in implicit and explicit memory. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 41(8), 694-705. |
[ 孟迎芳, 郭春彦 . (2009). 内隐与外显记忆的编码与提取非对称性关系. 心理学报, 41(8), 694-705.] | |
[25] | Meng, Y. F., & Lin, H. R . (2018). Attentional boost effect: New insights on relationship between attention and memory. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(2), 221-228. |
[ 孟迎芳, 林惠茹 . (2018). 注意促进效应: 注意与记忆关系的新见解. 心理科学进展, 26(2), 221-228.] | |
[26] | Meng, Y. F., Lin, G. Y., & Lin, H. R . (2018). The role of distractor inhibition in the attentional boost effect: Evidence from the R/K paradigm. Memory, 27(6), 750-757. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1563188URL |
[27] | Mulligan, N. W., Spataro, P., & Picklesimer, M . (2014). The attentional boost effect with verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(4), 1049-1063. doi: 10.1037/a0036163URL |
[28] | Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Brubaker, M. S . (2019). Are the effects of divided attention on memory encoding processes due to the disruption of deep-level elaborative processes? Evidence from cued- and free-recall tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 106, 108-117. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2019.02.007URL |
[29] | Naveh-Benjamin, M., Craik, F. I. M., Guez, J., & Kreuger, S . (2005). Divided attention in younger and older adults: Effects of strategy and relatedness on memory performance and secondary task costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 520-537. |
[30] | Olds, J. M., & Westerman, D. L . (2012). Can fluency be interpreted as novelty? Retraining the interpretation of fluency in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 653-664. |
[31] | Pape, A.-A., Noury, N., & Siegel, M . (2017). Motor actions influence subsequent sensorimotor decisions. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 15913. |
[32] | Pashler, H . (1994). Graded capacity-sharing in dual-task interference? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(2), 330-342. |
[33] | Rossi-Arnaud, C., Spataro, P., Saraulli, D., Mulligan, N. W., Sciarretta, A., Marques, V. R. S., & Cestari, V . (2014). The attentional boost effect in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(3), 588-597. |
[34] | Spataro, P., Mulligan, N. W., & Rossi-Arnaud, C . (2013). Divided attention can enhance memory encoding: The attentional boost effect in implicit memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(4), 1223-1231. |
[35] | Swallow, K. M., & Jiang, Y. V . (2010). The attentional boost effect: Transient increases in attention to one task enhance performance in a second task. Cognition, 115(1), 118-132. |
[36] | Swallow, K. M., & Jiang, Y. V . (2011). The role of timing in the attentional boost effect. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(2), 389-404. |
[37] | Swallow, K. M., & Jiang, Y. V . (2012). Goal-relevant events need not be rare to boost memory for concurrent images. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 74(1), 70-82. |
[38] | Swallow, K. M., & Jiang, Y. V . (2013). Attentional load and attentional boost: A review of data and theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 274. |
[39] | Swallow, K. M., Makovski, T., & Jiang, Y. V . (2012). Selection of events in time enhances activity throughout early visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108(12), 3239-3252. |
[40] | Westerman, D. L., Lloyd, M. E., & Miller, J. K . (2002). The attribution of perceptual fluency in recognition memory: The role of expectation. Journal of Memory & Language, 47(4), 607-617. |
[41] | Whittlesea , & Bruce, W. A . (2002). False memory and the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: The prototype- familiarity illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(1), 96-115. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.96URL |
[42] | Wolpert, D. M., & Landy, M. S . (2012). Motor control is decision-making. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22(6), 996-1003. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.05.003URL |
[43] | Wurtz, P., Reber, R., & Zimmermann, T. D . (2008). The feeling of fluent perception: A single experience from multiple asynchronous sources. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(1), 171-184. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.07.001URL |
相关文章 15
[1] | 孟迎芳, 叶秀敏, 马慧姣. 分类学习与混合学习下的注意促进效应比较[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(2): 139-148. |
[2] | 张丹丹,蔺义芹,柳昀哲,罗跃嘉,蒋冬红. 厌恶与恐惧面孔的记忆编码、保持、提取[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(1): 36-47. |
[3] | 林无忌, 孟迎芳, 林静远. 提取干扰对内隐记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(7): 897-908. |
[4] | 黄碧娟;封洪敏;司继伟;张杰;王翔艳. 双任务协调、任务呈现方式影响成人算术策略执行:估算证据[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(6): 671-683. |
[5] | 艾继如;张红段;司继伟;卢淳;张堂正. 任务呈现方式、双任务反应顺序影响算术估算策略选择与执行[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(10): 1248-1257. |
[6] | 魏柳青;张学民;李永娜;马玉. 视听通道双任务对多目标追踪的影响:干扰还是促进?[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(6): 727-739. |
[7] | 吴彦文;游旭群;李海霞. 注意力资源限制与双任务的相互干扰机制[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(2): 174-184. |
[8] | 尹华站;李丹;袁祥勇;黄希庭. 跨通道情境下双任务范式与计时中断范式中的效应比较[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(8): 860-873. |
[9] | 谭金凤;伍姗姗;王小影;王丽君;赵远方;陈安涛. 奖励驱动的双任务加工过程中的分离脑机制:来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(3): 285-297. |
[10] | 游旭群,张媛,刘登攀. 仿真场景下类别空间关系判断中的注意分配[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(07): 759-765. |
[11] | 樊春雷,张爱玲. 品牌态度强度与品牌态度可达性的关系性质[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(2): 355-361. |
[12] | 张明,张阳,付佳. 工作记忆对动态范式中基于客体的返回抑制的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(01): 35-42. |
[13] | 马淑蕾,黄敏儿 . 情绪劳动:表层动作与深层动作,哪一种效果更好?[J]. 心理学报, 2006, 38(02): 262-270. |
[14] | 陈湘川,解恒革,张达人,王鲁宁,陈隆隆,唐孝威. 早老性痴呆病人早期记忆损害的检测方法[J]. 心理学报, 2001, 33(6): 69-72. |
[15] | 孙向红,郭素梅,张侃. 次要任务的输入方式对操作绩效和心理负荷的影响[J]. 心理学报, 1998, 30(3): 343-347. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4716