1 辽宁师范大学心理学院; 辽宁省儿童青少年健康人格评定与培养协同创新中心, 大连 116029
2 中国民用航空飞行学院航空人因与工效学研究所, 四川 广汉 418307
收稿日期:
2019-08-08出版日期:
2020-03-25发布日期:
2020-01-18通讯作者:
唐晓雨E-mail:tangyu-2006@163.com基金资助:
* 国家自然科学基金项目资助(31600882)The effect of bimodal divided attention on inhibition of return with audiovisual targets
TANG Xiaoyu1(), SUN Jiaying1, PENG Xing21 School of Psychology, Liaoning Collaborative Innovation Center of Children and Adolescents Healthy Personality Assessment and Cultivation, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
2 Institute of Aviation Human Factors and Ergonomics, Civil Aviation Flight University of China, Guanghan 618307, China
Received:
2019-08-08Online:
2020-03-25Published:
2020-01-18Contact:
TANG Xiaoyu E-mail:tangyu-2006@163.com摘要/Abstract
摘要: 本研究基于线索-靶子范式, 操纵目标刺激类型(视觉、听觉、视听觉)与线索有效性(有效线索、中性条件、无效线索)两个自变量, 通过3个实验来考察双通道分配性注意对视听觉返回抑制(inhibition of return, IOR)的影响。实验1 (听觉刺激呈现在左/右侧)结果发现, 在双通道分配性注意条件下, 视觉目标产生显著IOR效应, 而视听觉目标没有产生IOR效应; 实验2 (听觉刺激呈现在左/右侧)与实验3 (听觉刺激呈现在中央)结果发现, 在视觉通道选择性注意条件下, 视觉与视听觉目标均产生显著IOR效应但二者无显著差异。结果表明:双通道分配性注意减弱视听觉IOR效应。
图/表 7
图1实验1流程示意图 注:图左为实验刺激呈现位置的示意图, 图右为单个试次的流程图。图右中视觉线索(白色方框)呈现在左侧, 目标(视听觉)也呈现在左侧(即, 有效线索位置), 要求被试对目标刺激进行既快又准的检测反应。其中, 目标刺激(V/A/AV)分别代表视觉(visual)、听觉(auditory)和视听觉(audiovisual)通道目标。ISI是指刺激间时间间隔(inter-stimulus interval)。ITI是指试次间的时间间隔(inter-trial interval)。
图1实验1流程示意图 注:图左为实验刺激呈现位置的示意图, 图右为单个试次的流程图。图右中视觉线索(白色方框)呈现在左侧, 目标(视听觉)也呈现在左侧(即, 有效线索位置), 要求被试对目标刺激进行既快又准的检测反应。其中, 目标刺激(V/A/AV)分别代表视觉(visual)、听觉(auditory)和视听觉(audiovisual)通道目标。ISI是指刺激间时间间隔(inter-stimulus interval)。ITI是指试次间的时间间隔(inter-trial interval)。
表1实验1~3不同条件下的正确率与反应时(M±SD)
目标刺激类型 | 线索有效性 | 实验1 | 实验2 | 实验3 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ACC (%) | RT (ms) | ACC (%) | RT (ms) | ACC (%) | RT (ms) | ||
AV | 有效线索 | 99 ± 1 | 449 ± 61 | 99 ± 1 | 418 ± 37 | 97 ± 5 | 425 ± 77 |
无效线索 | 99 ± 1 | 446 ± 65 | 99 ± 1 | 401 ± 35 | 96 ± 6 | 408 ± 79 | |
V | 有效线索 | 97 ± 3 | 476 ± 63 | 98 ± 2 | 432 ± 37 | 95 ± 9 | 452 ± 81 |
无效线索 | 97 ± 3 | 465 ± 70 | 99 ± 2 | 432 ± 36 | 95 ± 7 | 431 ± 80 | |
A | 有效线索 | 96 ± 2 | 498 ± 73 | — | — | — | — |
无效线索 | 94 ± 4 | 519 ± 83 | — | — | — | — |
表1实验1~3不同条件下的正确率与反应时(M±SD)
目标刺激类型 | 线索有效性 | 实验1 | 实验2 | 实验3 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ACC (%) | RT (ms) | ACC (%) | RT (ms) | ACC (%) | RT (ms) | ||
AV | 有效线索 | 99 ± 1 | 449 ± 61 | 99 ± 1 | 418 ± 37 | 97 ± 5 | 425 ± 77 |
无效线索 | 99 ± 1 | 446 ± 65 | 99 ± 1 | 401 ± 35 | 96 ± 6 | 408 ± 79 | |
V | 有效线索 | 97 ± 3 | 476 ± 63 | 98 ± 2 | 432 ± 37 | 95 ± 9 | 452 ± 81 |
无效线索 | 97 ± 3 | 465 ± 70 | 99 ± 2 | 432 ± 36 | 95 ± 7 | 431 ± 80 | |
A | 有效线索 | 96 ± 2 | 498 ± 73 | — | — | — | — |
无效线索 | 94 ± 4 | 519 ± 83 | — | — | — | — |
图2实验1不同目标刺激类型和线索有效性下的平均反应时(*p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001)
图2实验1不同目标刺激类型和线索有效性下的平均反应时(*p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001)
表2实验1~3不同条件下CE、rMRE结果对比
实验 | 条件 | M | 95% CI | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
下限 | 上限 | |||||
实验1 | ||||||
CE | V | 10.83 | 1.15 | 20.52 | 2.27 | 0.029 |
A | -21.29 | -30.92 | -11.66 | -4.50 | 0.000 | |
AV | 2.78 | -6.27 | 11.84 | 0.62 | 0.538 | |
CE对比 | AV vs. V | -8.05 | -14.83 | -1.26 | -2.41 | 0.022 |
rMRE | 有效 | 4.08 | 2.90 | 5.27 | 7.01 | 0.000 |
无效 | 2.38 | 0.88 | 3.87 | 3.24 | 0.003 | |
rMRE对比 | 有效vs.无效 | 1.70 | 0.24 | 3.16 | 2.38 | 0.023 |
实验2 | ||||||
CE | V | 9.47 | 0.68 | 18.26 | 2.20 | 0.036 |
AV | 17.25 | 7.14 | 27.35 | 3.49 | 0.002 | |
CE对比 | AV vs. V | 7.78 | -0.91 | 16.46 | 1.83 | 0.077 |
rMRE | 有效 | 2.01 | 0.25 | 3.76 | 2.33 | 0.026 |
无效 | 3.42 | 1.42 | 5.42 | 3.50 | 0.002 | |
rMRE对比 | 有效vs.无效 | -1.41 | -3.51 | 0.68 | -1.37 | 0.178 |
实验3 | ||||||
CE | V | 20.35 | 5.62 | 35.08 | 2.80 | 0.008 |
AV | 17.02 | 4.18 | 29.86 | 2.68 | 0.011 | |
CE对比 | AV vs. V | -3.33 | -13.94 | 7.27 | -0.63 | 0.528 |
rMRE | 有效 | 4.75 | 2.96 | 6.55 | 5.36 | 0.000 |
无效 | 4.89 | 2.75 | 7.04 | 4.62 | 0.000 | |
rMRE对比 | 有效vs.无效 | -0.14 | -2.25 | 1.97 | -0.13 | 0.893 |
表2实验1~3不同条件下CE、rMRE结果对比
实验 | 条件 | M | 95% CI | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
下限 | 上限 | |||||
实验1 | ||||||
CE | V | 10.83 | 1.15 | 20.52 | 2.27 | 0.029 |
A | -21.29 | -30.92 | -11.66 | -4.50 | 0.000 | |
AV | 2.78 | -6.27 | 11.84 | 0.62 | 0.538 | |
CE对比 | AV vs. V | -8.05 | -14.83 | -1.26 | -2.41 | 0.022 |
rMRE | 有效 | 4.08 | 2.90 | 5.27 | 7.01 | 0.000 |
无效 | 2.38 | 0.88 | 3.87 | 3.24 | 0.003 | |
rMRE对比 | 有效vs.无效 | 1.70 | 0.24 | 3.16 | 2.38 | 0.023 |
实验2 | ||||||
CE | V | 9.47 | 0.68 | 18.26 | 2.20 | 0.036 |
AV | 17.25 | 7.14 | 27.35 | 3.49 | 0.002 | |
CE对比 | AV vs. V | 7.78 | -0.91 | 16.46 | 1.83 | 0.077 |
rMRE | 有效 | 2.01 | 0.25 | 3.76 | 2.33 | 0.026 |
无效 | 3.42 | 1.42 | 5.42 | 3.50 | 0.002 | |
rMRE对比 | 有效vs.无效 | -1.41 | -3.51 | 0.68 | -1.37 | 0.178 |
实验3 | ||||||
CE | V | 20.35 | 5.62 | 35.08 | 2.80 | 0.008 |
AV | 17.02 | 4.18 | 29.86 | 2.68 | 0.011 | |
CE对比 | AV vs. V | -3.33 | -13.94 | 7.27 | -0.63 | 0.528 |
rMRE | 有效 | 4.75 | 2.96 | 6.55 | 5.36 | 0.000 |
无效 | 4.89 | 2.75 | 7.04 | 4.62 | 0.000 | |
rMRE对比 | 有效vs.无效 | -0.14 | -2.25 | 1.97 | -0.13 | 0.893 |
图3实验1不同线索有效性下的rMRE 注:rMRE (相对多感觉反应增强; relative amount of multisensory response enhancement); *p < 0.05。
图3实验1不同线索有效性下的rMRE 注:rMRE (相对多感觉反应增强; relative amount of multisensory response enhancement); *p < 0.05。
图4实验2不同条件下的平均反应时和rMRE 注:(a)不同目标刺激类型和线索有效性下的平均反应时(*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01)。(b)不同线索有效性下的rMRE (相对多感觉反应增强; relative amount of multisensory response enhancement)。
图4实验2不同条件下的平均反应时和rMRE 注:(a)不同目标刺激类型和线索有效性下的平均反应时(*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01)。(b)不同线索有效性下的rMRE (相对多感觉反应增强; relative amount of multisensory response enhancement)。
图5实验3不同条件下的平均反应时和rMRE 注:(a)不同目标刺激类型和线索有效性下的平均反应时(*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01)。(b)不同线索有效性下的rMRE (相对多感觉反应增强; relative amount of multisensory response enhancement)。
图5实验3不同条件下的平均反应时和rMRE 注:(a)不同目标刺激类型和线索有效性下的平均反应时(*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01)。(b)不同线索有效性下的rMRE (相对多感觉反应增强; relative amount of multisensory response enhancement)。
参考文献 39
[1] | Beck D. M., & Kastner S . (2009). Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain. Vision Research, 49(10), 1154-1165. |
[2] | Carrasco M . (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13), 1484-1525. |
[3] | Chica A. B., Lupianez J., & Bartolomeo P . (2006). Dissociating inhibition of return from endogenous orienting of spatial attention: Evidence from detection and discrimination tasks. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(7), 1015-1034. |
[4] | Eimer M., & Driver J . (2001). Crossmodal links in endogenous and exogenous spatial attention: Evidence from event-related brain potential studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 25(6), 497-511. |
[5] | Frassinetti F., Bolognini N., & Làdavas E . (2002). Enhancement of visual perception by crossmodal visuo-auditory interaction. Experimental Brain Research, 147(3), 332-343. |
[6] | Gao Y., Li Q., Yang W., Yang J., Tang X., & Wu J . (2014). Effects of ipsilateral and bilateral auditory stimuli on audiovisual integration: A behavioral and event-related potential study. Neuroreport, 25(9), 668-675. |
[7] | Klein R . (1988). Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search. Nature, 334(6181), 430-431. |
[8] | Klein R. M . (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 138-147. |
[9] | Lupiáñez J., Milán E. G., Tornay F. J., Madrid E., & Tudela P . (1997). Does IOR occur in discrimination tasks? Yes, it does, but later. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(8), 1241-1254. |
[10] | Lupiáñez J., Ruz M., Funes M. J., & Milliken B . (2007). The manifestation of attentional capture: Facilitation or IOR depending on task demands. Psychological Research, 71(1), 77-91. |
[11] | Matusz P. J., & Eimer M . (2011). Multisensory enhancement of attentional capture in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(5), 904-909. |
[12] | Mcdonald J. J., & Ward L. M . (1999). Spatial relevance determines facilitatory and inhibitory effects of auditory covert spatial orienting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 25(5), 1234-1252. |
[13] | Meredith M. A., & Stein B. E . (1986). Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. Journal of Neurophysiology, 56(3), 640-662. |
[14] | Mishra J., Bavelier D., & Gazzaley A . (2012). How to assess gaming-induced benefits on attention and working memory. Games for Health Journal, 1(3), 192-198. |
[15] | Mozolic J. L., Hugenschmidt C. E., Peiffer A. M., & Laurienti P. J . (2008). Modality-specific selective attention attenuates multisensory integration. Experimental Brain Research, 184(1), 39-52. |
[16] | Peng X., Chang R S., Li Q., Wang A J., & Tang X Y . (2019). Visually induced inhibition of return affects the audiovisual integration under different SOA conditions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(7), 759-771. |
[ 彭姓, 常若松, 李奇, 王爱君, 唐晓雨 . (2019). 不同SOA下视觉返回抑制对视听觉整合的调节作用. 心理学报, 51(7), 759-771.] | |
[17] | Posner M. I., & Cohen Y . (1984). Components of visual orienting. Attention and performance X: Control of Language Processes, 32, 531-556. |
[18] | Posner M. I., Rafal R. D., Choate L. S., & Vaughan J . (1985). Inhibition of return: Neural basis and function. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2(3), 211-228. |
[19] | Pratt J., & Fischer M. H . (2002). Examining the role of the fixation cue in inhibition of return. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 56(4), 294-301. |
[20] | Pratt J., Kingstone A., & Khoe W . (1997). Inhibition of return in location- and identity-based choice decision tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(6), 964-971. |
[21] | Prime D. J., Tata M. S., & Ward L. M . (2003). Event-related potential evidence for attentional inhibition of return in audition. Neuroreport, 14(3), 393-397. |
[22] | Reuter-Lorenz P. A., & Rosenquist J. N . (1996). Auditory cues and inhibition of return: The importance of oculomotor activation. Experimental Brain Research, 112(1), 119-126. |
[23] | Roggeveen A. B., Prime D. J., & Ward L. M . (2005). Inhibition of return and response repetition within and between modalities. Experimental Brain Research, 167(1), 86-94. |
[24] | Santangelo V., & Spence C . (2007). Multisensory cues capture spatial attention regardless of perceptual load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1311-1321. |
[25] | Satel J., Hilchey M. D., Wang Z. G., Story R., & Klein R. M . (2013). The effects of ignored versus foveated cues upon inhibition of return: An event-related potential study. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(1), 29-40. |
[26] | Schmitt M., Postma A., & de Haan E . (2000). Interactions between exogenous auditory and visual spatial attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 53(1), 105-130. |
[27] | Senkowski D., Saint-Amour D., Höfle M., & Foxe J. J . (2011). Multisensory interactions in early evoked brain activity follow the principle of inverse effectiveness. Neuroimage, 56(4), 2200-2208. |
[28] | Slagter H. A., Prinssen S., Reteig L. C., & Mazaheri A . (2016). Facilitation and inhibition in attention: Functional dissociation of pre-stimulus alpha activity, P1, and N1 components. Neuroimage, 125(6), 25-35. |
[29] | Spence C., & Driver J . (1998). Inhibition of return following an auditory cue. The role of central reorienting events. Experimental Brain Research, 118(3), 352-360. |
[30] | Spence C., Lloyd D., Mcglone F., Nicholls M. E. R., & Driver J . (2000). Inhibition of return is supramodal: A demonstration between all possible pairings of vision, touch, and audition. Experimental Brain Research, 134(1), 42-48. |
[31] | Talsma D., & Woldorff M. G . (2005). Selective attention and multisensory integration: Multiple phases of effects on the evoked brain activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(7), 1098-1114. |
[32] | Tang X., Gao Y., Yang W., Ren Y., Wu J., Ming Z., & Wu Q . (2019). Bimodal divided attention attenuates inhibition of return with audiovisual targets. Experimental Brain Research, 237(4), 1093-1107. |
[33] | Tang X., Wu J., & Shen Y . (2016). The interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and exogenous attention. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 61, 208-224. |
[34] | van der Burg E., Olivers C. N. L., Bronkhorst A. W., & Theeuwes J . (2008). Pip and pop: Nonspatial auditory signals improve spatial visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(5), 1053-1065. |
[35] | van der Burg E., Talsma D., Olivers C. N., Hickey C., & Theeuwes J . (2011). Early multisensory interactions affect the competition among multiple visual objects. Neuroimage, 55(3), 1208-1218. |
[36] | van der Stoep N., van der Stigchel S., & Nijboer T. C. W . (2015). Exogenous spatial attention decreases audiovisual integration. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(2), 464-482. |
[37] | van der Stoep N., van der Stigchel S., Nijboer T. C. W., & Spence C . (2016). Visually induced inhibition of return affects the integration of auditory and visual information. Perception, 46(1), 6-17. |
[38] | Wu J., Yang J., Yu Y., Li Q., Nakamura N., Shen Y., … Abe K . (2012). Delayed audiovisual integration of patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease compared with normal aged controls. Journal of Alzheimers Disease Jad, 32(2), 317-328. |
[39] | Zhang M., Tang X., & Wu J . (2013). Blocking the link between stimulus and response at previously attended locations: Evidence for inhibitory tagging mechanism. Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, 1(1), 13-21. |
相关文章 15
[1] | 张明, 桑汉斌, 鲁柯, 王爱君. 试次历史对跨通道非空间返回抑制的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 681-693. |
[2] | 唐晓雨, 吴英楠, 彭姓, 王爱君, 李奇. 内源性空间线索有效性对视听觉整合的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 835-846. |
[3] | 彭姓,常若松,李奇,王爱君,唐晓雨. 不同SOA下视觉返回抑制对视听觉整合的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(7): 759-771. |
[4] | 王爱君, 刘晓乐, 唐晓雨, 张 明. 三维空间中不同视野深度位置上的返回抑制[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(6): 723-732. |
[5] | 徐菊;胡媛艳;王双; 李艾苏;张明;张阳. 返回抑制训练效应的认知神经机制 ——来自ERP研究的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(6): 658-670. |
[6] | 徐菊;马方圆;张明;张阳. 返回抑制和抑制标签在长时训练下的分离[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(8): 981-991. |
[7] | 王爱君;李毕琴;张明. 三维空间深度位置上基于空间的返回抑制[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(7): 859-868. |
[8] | 范海楠;许百华. 动态情景中颜色特征和身份特征在返回抑制中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(11): 1628-1638. |
[9] | 张瑜;郑希付;黄珊珊;李悦;杜晓芬;周薇. 不同线索下特质焦虑个体的返回抑制[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(4): 446-452. |
[10] | 徐丹妮;张佳悦;李先春. 面孔性别辨认中返回抑制效应的性别差异[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(2): 161-168. |
[11] | 王敬欣;贾丽萍;白学军;罗跃嘉. 返回抑制过程中情绪面孔加工优先:ERPs研究[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(1): 1-10. |
[12] | 刘盼,谢宁,吴艳红. 认知老化中有意控制对自动抑制的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2010, 42(10): 981-987. |
[13] | 邓晓红,张德玄,黄诗雪,袁,雯,周晓林. 阈上和阈下不同情绪线索对返回抑制的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2010, 42(03): 325-333. |
[14] | 戴琴,冯正直. 抑郁个体对情绪面孔的返回抑制能力不足[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(12): 1175-1188. |
[15] | 沈模卫,高在峰,张光强,水仁德,乔歆新,李伟健. 三维倾斜平面的返回抑制[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(06): 951-958. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4654