删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

数字有形状吗?数字信息精确性和品牌标识形状的匹配效应

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

杨晨1, 陈增祥2()
1 华南理工大学工商管理学院, 广州 510641
2 中山大学国际金融学院, 珠海 519082
收稿日期:2018-03-20出版日期:2019-07-25发布日期:2019-05-22
通讯作者:陈增祥E-mail:chzengx@mail.sysu.edu.cn

基金资助:* 国家自然科学基金项目(71502067);国家自然科学基金项目(71572084);国家自然科学基金项目(71802202);国家自然科学基金项目(71832015);国家自然科学基金项目(71802051);广东省自然科学基金博士启动项目(2016A030310425);教育部哲学社会科学研究重大课题攻关项目资助(17JZD020)

Do numbers have shape? The matching effect between precise numerical information and brand logo shape

YANG Chen1, CHEN Zeng-Xiang2()
1 School of Business Administration, South China University of Technology, Guangzhou 510641, China
2 International School of Business & Finance, Sun Yat-sen University, Zhuhai 519082, China
Received:2018-03-20Online:2019-07-25Published:2019-05-22
Contact:CHEN Zeng-Xiang E-mail:chzengx@mail.sysu.edu.cn






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 数字的精确性(精确 vs.大概)会影响人们的各种心理与推断, 本文在已有研究的基础上提出数字精确性与形状之间存在关联。具体而言, 精确数字会更有可能被感知为多角, 而大概数字更可能被感知为圆润。实验1和2分别通过内隐联想测试和消费情境确认这一效应的存在, 实验3进一步探讨了流畅性感知对上述匹配效应影响消费者产品评价的中介作用, 实验4则通过反转数字的性别象征意义, 提出了匹配效应的边界条件:当人们头脑中形成“精确=女性、大概=男性”的联系时, 会产生精确数字-圆润形状和大概数字-多角形状的匹配关系。这些结果说明了数字信息与形状信息之间存在认知关联, 它对理解个体如何加工数字信息、形状信息以及企业如何利用这些信息提供了一定的启发意义。



图1实验1不同形状背景下被试对不同数字信息的平均反应时(左)和平均正确率(右) (M ± SD) 注:**p < 0.01
图1实验1不同形状背景下被试对不同数字信息的平均反应时(左)和平均正确率(右) (M ± SD) 注:**p < 0.01



图2实验2中产品数字信息和品牌标识对消费者品牌态度的影响(M ± SD) 注:* p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001
图2实验2中产品数字信息和品牌标识对消费者品牌态度的影响(M ± SD) 注:* p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001



图3实验3中产品数字信息和品牌标识对消费者品牌态度的影响(M ± SD) 注:**p < 0.01
图3实验3中产品数字信息和品牌标识对消费者品牌态度的影响(M ± SD) 注:**p < 0.01



图4实验3中广告信息流畅性感知的中介示意图 注:**p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
图4实验3中广告信息流畅性感知的中介示意图 注:**p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001



图5不同数字-性别联结模式下(“精确=女性” (左图)和“精确=男性” (右图))数字信息和品牌标识对产品评价的影响(M ± SD) 注:*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01
图5不同数字-性别联结模式下(“精确=女性” (左图)和“精确=男性” (右图))数字信息和品牌标识对产品评价的影响(M ± SD) 注:*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01







[1] Anderson M. L . ( 2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 33( 4), 275-276.
[2] Arnheim R. , (2010). Entropy and art : An essay on disorder and order: University of California Press.
[3] Backus M., Blake T., & Tadelis S . ( 2015). Cheap talk, round numbers, and the economics of negotiation. NBER Working Paper, W21285.
[4] Cai F., Shen H., & Hui M. K . ( 2012). The effect of location on price estimation: Understanding number-location and number-order associations. Journal of Marketing Research, 49( 5), 718-724.
[5] Chae B. & Hoegg J. ,( 2013). The future looks “Right”: Effects of the horizontal location of advertising images on product attitude. Journal of Consumer Research, 40( 2), 223-238.
[6] Dehaene S., Bossini S., & Giraux P . ( 1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122( 3), 371-396.
[7] Feng W. T., & Wang T.,( 2017). The power of numbers: The influence of number magnitude in brands on consumers' attitudes. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49( 12), 1581-1589.
[ 冯文婷, 汪涛 . ( 2017). 数字的力量: 品牌中数字大小对消费者态度的影响. 心理学报, 49( 12), 1581-1589. ]
[8] Greenwald A. G., Nosek B. A., & Banaji M. R . ( 2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85( 2), 197-216.
[9] Hagtvedt H. , (2011). The impact of incomplete typeface logos on perceptions of the firm. Journal of Marketing, 75( 4), 86-93.
[10] Hayes A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press.
[11] Hevner K.. , ( 1935). Experimental studies of the affective value of colors and lines. Journal of Applied Psychology, 19( 4), 385-398.
[12] Isaac M. S., & Schindler R. M . ( 2014). The Top-Ten Effect: Consumers' subjective categorization of ranked lists. Journal of Consumer Research, 40( 6), 1181-1202.
[13] Janiszewski C., & Uy D. , ( 2008). Precision of the anchor influences the amount of adjustment. Psychological Science, 19( 2), 121-127.
[14] Jerez-Fernandez A., Angulo A. N., & Oppenheimer D. M . ( 2014). Show me the numbers: Precision as a cue to others’ confidence. Psychological Science, 25( 2), 633-635.
[15] Jia M., Krishna A., & Li X . ( 2017). The effect of visual cues on the sense of completeness. Advances in Consumer Research.45, 286-290.
[16] Jiang Y., Gorn G. J., Galli M., & Chattopadhyay A . ( 2016). Does your company have the right logo? How and why circular-and angular-logo shapes influence brand attribute judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 42( 5), 709-726.
[17] King D., & Janiszewski C. , ( 2011). The sources and consequences of the fluent processing of numbers. Journal of Marketing Research, 48( 2), 327-341.
doi: 10.1509/jmkr.48.2.327URL
[18] Lee A. Y., & Aaker J. L . ( 2004). Bringing the frame Into focus: The Influence of regulatory fit on processing fluency and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86( 2), 205-218.
[19] Li Y. J., Haws K. L., & Griskevicius V . ( 2019). Parenting motivation and consumer decision-making. Journal of Consumer Research, 45( 5), 1117-1137.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucy038URL
[20] Liu S. Q., Bogicevic V., & Mattila A. S . ( 2018). Circular vs. angular servicescape: “Shaping” customer response to a fast service encounter pace. Journal of Business Research, 89, 47-56.
[21] Mehta R., & Zhu R. J . ( 2009). Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on cognitive task performances. Science, 323( 5918), 1226-1229.
[22] Messner C. & Vosgerau J. , ( 2010). Cognitive inertia and the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Marketing Research, 47( 2), 374-386.
[23] Moshe B. & Maital N. , ( 2006). Humans prefer curved visual objects. Psychological Science, 17( 8), 645-648.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01759.xURL
[24] Palumbo L., Ruta N., & Bertamini M . ( 2015). Comparing angular and curved shapes in terms of implicit associations and approach/avoidance responses. PLoS ONE, 10( 10), e140043.
[25] Pang X. J., Zhang D. J., Wang X. Q., & Wang J. L . ( 2011). Review on intervention strategy studies about stereotype. Advances in Psychological Science, 19( 2), 243-250.
[ 庞小佳, 张大均, 王鑫强, 王金良 . ( 2011). 刻板印象干预策略研究述评. 心理科学进展, 19( 2), 243-250.]
[26] Pena-Marin J. & Bhargave R. , ( 2016). Lasting performance: Round numbers activate associations of stability and increase perceived length of product benefits. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26( 3), 410-416.
[27] Qian M., Zhou L. X., Lu T. T Wen, M. X. & Fu, G. Y. ., ( 2015). Theoretical construction and empirical research on a preschooler-friendly Implicit Association Test, Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47( 7), 903-913.
[ 钱淼, 周立霞, 鲁甜甜, 翁梦星, 傅根跃 . ( 2015). 幼儿友好型内隐联想测验的建构及有效性. 心理学报, 47( 7), 903-913.]
[28] Reber R., Schwarz N., & Winkielman P . ( 2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure:Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8( 4), 364-382.
[29] Rozin P., Hormes J. M., Faith M. S., & Wansink B . ( 2012). Is meat male? A quantitative multimethod framework to establish metaphoric relationships. Journal of Consumer Research, 39( 3), 629-643.
[30] Schindler R. M., & Yalch R. F . ( 2006). It seems factual, But Is It? Effects of using sharp versus round numbers in advertising claims. Advances in Consumer Research, 33( 1), 586-590.
[31] Shoham M., Moldovan S., & Steinhart Y . ( 2018). Mind the Gap: How smaller numerical differences can increase product attractiveness. Journal of Consumer Research, 45( 4), 761-774.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucy022URL
[32] Thomas M., Simon D. H., & Kadiyal V . ( 2010). The price precision effect: Evidence from laboratory and market data. Marketing Science, 29( 1), 175-190.
doi: 10.1287/mksc.1090.0512URL
[33] Touré-Tillery M. & Fishbach A. , ( 2017). Too far to help: The effect of perceived distance on the expected impact and likelihood of charitable action. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 112( 6), 860-876.
[34] Wang H. Z., Fang X. W., & Ouyang J. Y . ( 2017). Consumer self-construal, need of uniqueness and preference of brand logo shape. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49( 8), 1113-1124.
[ 王海忠, 范孝雯, 欧阳建颖 . ( 2017). 消费者自我构念, 独特性需求与品牌标识形状偏好. 心理学报, 49( 8), 1113-1124.]
[35] Wadhwa M. & Zhang K. ,( 2015). This number just feels right: The impact of roundedness of price numbers on product evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 41( 5), 1172-1185.
[36] Westerman S. J., Gardner P. H., Sutherland E. J., White T., Jordan K., Watts D., & Wells S . ( 2012). Product design: Preference for rounded versus angular design elements. Psychology & Marketing, 29( 8), 595-605.
[37] Xie G. X., & Kronrod A. , ( 2012). Is the devil in the details? The signaling effect of numerical precision in environmental advertising claims. Journal of Advertising, 41( 4), 103-117.
[38] Xu X. D., & Liu C.,( 2006). The spatial character of number. Advances in Psychological Science, 14( 6), 851-858.
[ 徐晓东, 刘昌 . ( 2006). 数字的空间特性. 心理科学进展, 14( 6), 851-858.]
[39] Yan D. , ( 2016). Numbers are gendered: The role of numerical precision. Journal of Consumer Research, 43( 2), 303-316.
[40] Yan D. & Pena-Marin J. , ( 2017). Round off the bargaining: The effects of offer roundness on willingness to accept. Journal of Consumer Research. 44( 2), 381-395.
[41] Zeng X. H . ( 2007). The cognition research about different number surface forms. Master’s thesis of South China Normal University.
[ 曾细红 . ( 2007). 不同的数字表面形式的认知研究 (硕士学位论文), 华南师范大学. ]
[42] Zhang Y., Feick L., & Price L. J . ( 2006). The impact of self-construal on aesthetic preference for angular versus rounded shapes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32( 6), 794-805.
[43] Zhu R., & Argo J. J . ( 2013). Exploring the impact of various shaped seating arrangements on persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 40( 2), 336-349.




[1]王海忠, 范孝雯, 欧阳建颖. 消费者自我构念、独特性需求与 品牌标识形状偏好[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(8): 1113-1124.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4478
相关话题/数字 信息 实验 心理 科学