天津师范大学教育科学学院, 天津 300387
收稿日期:
2017-05-02出版日期:
2019-01-25发布日期:
2018-11-26通讯作者:
刘希平基金资助:
* 天津市哲学社会科学规划课题资助(TJJX15-017);天津师范大学培育国家社科重大招标项目专项基金资助(2017)Effects of facial attractiveness and information accuracy on preschoolers’ selective trust
TANG Weihai, ZHONG Rubo, XU Xiaoxu, LIU Xiping*School of Educational Science, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300387, China
Received:
2017-05-02Online:
2019-01-25Published:
2018-11-26Contact:
LIU Xiping 摘要/Abstract
摘要: 实验1采用3×2×2 的混合设计, 考察4~6岁幼儿获取全新信息时基于面孔吸引力的选择性信任及其特点。结果显示:(1) 4~6岁的男孩和女孩均表现出对高面孔吸引力者的选择性信任。(2)在选择性信任程度方面:5岁比4岁高, 而4岁和6岁之间、5岁和6岁之间则相同; 女孩在询问任务中比男孩表现出更高的选择性信任, 在赞同任务中则相同。那么, 幼儿的这种选择性信任是否受到信息正确性的调节?为此, 实验2采用2×3的被试间设计, 探讨信息提供者提供信息的正确性对4~6岁幼儿基于面孔吸引力的选择性信任的影响。结果显示:在正确性为50% vs. 50%条件下, 幼儿未对信息提供者产生选择性信任; 在正确性为25% vs. 75%条件下, 幼儿对提供较大正确性信息的低面孔吸引力者产生了选择性信任。研究结果表明:(1)在信息缺乏时, 4~6岁幼儿普遍存在有偏见的选择性信任; 女孩比男孩更易受到社会偏见因素的影响。(2)在信息明确时, 4~6岁幼儿也能做出相对理性的选择性信任。
图/表 9
表1被试基本信息
年龄 | 性别 | 人数(n) | 月龄 | 月龄范围 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | 最小值 | 最大值 | |||
4岁 | 男 | 33 | 54.24 | 2.16 | 52.08 | 56.40 |
女 | 30 | 55.20 | 1.80 | 53.40 | 57.00 | |
5岁 | 男 | 33 | 65.16 | 2.88 | 62.28 | 68.04 |
女 | 33 | 66.12 | 2.64 | 63.48 | 68.76 | |
6岁 | 男 | 33 | 77.88 | 2.04 | 75.84 | 79.92 |
女 | 33 | 78.24 | 2.04 | 76.20 | 80.28 |
表1被试基本信息
年龄 | 性别 | 人数(n) | 月龄 | 月龄范围 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | 最小值 | 最大值 | |||
4岁 | 男 | 33 | 54.24 | 2.16 | 52.08 | 56.40 |
女 | 30 | 55.20 | 1.80 | 53.40 | 57.00 | |
5岁 | 男 | 33 | 65.16 | 2.88 | 62.28 | 68.04 |
女 | 33 | 66.12 | 2.64 | 63.48 | 68.76 | |
6岁 | 男 | 33 | 77.88 | 2.04 | 75.84 | 79.92 |
女 | 33 | 78.24 | 2.04 | 76.20 | 80.28 |
表24~6岁幼儿的询问率和赞同率与随机水平的比较
年龄 | 性别(n) | 询问率(M ± SD) | t | d | 赞同率(M ± SD) | t | d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
4岁 | 男(33) | 0.61 ± 0.28 | 2.21* | 0.39 | 0.61 ± 0.28 | 2.17* | 0.39 |
女(30) | 0.71 ± 0.23 | 4.98*** | 0.91 | 0.65 ± 0.28 | 3.03** | 0.54 | |
总计(63) | 0.65 ± 0.26 | 4.75*** | 0.58 | 0.63 ± 0.28 | 3.67*** | 0.46 | |
5岁 | 男(33) | 0.77 ± 0.21 | 7.49*** | 1.29 | 0.68 ± 0.23 | 4.63*** | 0.78 |
女(33) | 0.85 ± 0.21 | 9.53*** | 1.67 | 0.72 ± 0.26 | 4.76*** | 0.85 | |
总计(66) | 0.81 ± 0.21 | 11.90*** | 1.48 | 0.7 ± 0.24 | 6.67*** | 0.83 | |
6岁 | 男(33) | 0.66 ± 0.24 | 3.81*** | 0.67 | 0.65 ± 0.21 | 4.13*** | 0.71 |
女(33) | 0.83 ± 0.25 | 7.73*** | 1.32 | 0.7 ± 0.23 | 4.92*** | 0.87 | |
总计(66) | 0.75 ± 0.26 | 7.77*** | 0.96 | 0.67 ± 0.22 | 6.44*** | 0.77 |
表24~6岁幼儿的询问率和赞同率与随机水平的比较
年龄 | 性别(n) | 询问率(M ± SD) | t | d | 赞同率(M ± SD) | t | d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
4岁 | 男(33) | 0.61 ± 0.28 | 2.21* | 0.39 | 0.61 ± 0.28 | 2.17* | 0.39 |
女(30) | 0.71 ± 0.23 | 4.98*** | 0.91 | 0.65 ± 0.28 | 3.03** | 0.54 | |
总计(63) | 0.65 ± 0.26 | 4.75*** | 0.58 | 0.63 ± 0.28 | 3.67*** | 0.46 | |
5岁 | 男(33) | 0.77 ± 0.21 | 7.49*** | 1.29 | 0.68 ± 0.23 | 4.63*** | 0.78 |
女(33) | 0.85 ± 0.21 | 9.53*** | 1.67 | 0.72 ± 0.26 | 4.76*** | 0.85 | |
总计(66) | 0.81 ± 0.21 | 11.90*** | 1.48 | 0.7 ± 0.24 | 6.67*** | 0.83 | |
6岁 | 男(33) | 0.66 ± 0.24 | 3.81*** | 0.67 | 0.65 ± 0.21 | 4.13*** | 0.71 |
女(33) | 0.83 ± 0.25 | 7.73*** | 1.32 | 0.7 ± 0.23 | 4.92*** | 0.87 | |
总计(66) | 0.75 ± 0.26 | 7.77*** | 0.96 | 0.67 ± 0.22 | 6.44*** | 0.77 |
表34~6岁幼儿的询问量和赞同量(M ± SD)
年龄 | 性别(n) | 询问量 | 赞同量 |
---|---|---|---|
4岁 | 男(33) | 0.11 ± 0.28 | 0.11 ± 0.28 |
女(30) | 0.21 ± 0.23 | 0.15 ± 0.28 | |
5岁 | 男(33) | 0.27 ± 0.21 | 0.18 ± 0.23 |
女(33) | 0.35 ± 0.21 | 0.22 ± 0.26 | |
6岁 | 男(33) | 0.16 ± 0.24 | 0.15 ± 0.21 |
女(33) | 0.33 ± 0.25 | 0.20 ± 0.23 |
表34~6岁幼儿的询问量和赞同量(M ± SD)
年龄 | 性别(n) | 询问量 | 赞同量 |
---|---|---|---|
4岁 | 男(33) | 0.11 ± 0.28 | 0.11 ± 0.28 |
女(30) | 0.21 ± 0.23 | 0.15 ± 0.28 | |
5岁 | 男(33) | 0.27 ± 0.21 | 0.18 ± 0.23 |
女(33) | 0.35 ± 0.21 | 0.22 ± 0.26 | |
6岁 | 男(33) | 0.16 ± 0.24 | 0.15 ± 0.21 |
女(33) | 0.33 ± 0.25 | 0.20 ± 0.23 |
图1性别和任务类型的交互作用
图1性别和任务类型的交互作用
表4被试基本信息
年龄 | 性别 | 人数(n1, n2) | 正确性为50% vs. 50% | 正确性为25% vs. 75% | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
月龄 | 月龄范围 | 月龄 | 月龄范围 | ||||||||
M | SD | 最小值 | 最大值 | M | SD | 最小值 | 最大值 | ||||
4岁 | 男 | (14, 15) | 54.66 | 4.10 | 50.56 | 58.76 | 54.05 | 3.33 | 50.72 | 57.38 | |
女 | (20, 13) | 55.93 | 1.92 | 54.01 | 57.85 | 55.29 | 1.58 | 53.71 | 56.87 | ||
5岁 | 男 | (16, 16) | 68.75 | 2.76 | 65.99 | 71.51 | 64.81 | 3.53 | 61.28 | 68.34 | |
女 | (15, 12) | 66.75 | 3.07 | 63.68 | 69.82 | 64.59 | 3.29 | 61.30 | 67.88 | ||
6岁 | 男 | (15, 14) | 77.71 | 1.78 | 75.93 | 79.49 | 78.6 | 1.49 | 77.11 | 80.09 | |
女 | (12, 15) | 77.38 | 2.15 | 75.23 | 79.53 | 77.8 | 1.79 | 76.01 | 79.59 |
表4被试基本信息
年龄 | 性别 | 人数(n1, n2) | 正确性为50% vs. 50% | 正确性为25% vs. 75% | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
月龄 | 月龄范围 | 月龄 | 月龄范围 | ||||||||
M | SD | 最小值 | 最大值 | M | SD | 最小值 | 最大值 | ||||
4岁 | 男 | (14, 15) | 54.66 | 4.10 | 50.56 | 58.76 | 54.05 | 3.33 | 50.72 | 57.38 | |
女 | (20, 13) | 55.93 | 1.92 | 54.01 | 57.85 | 55.29 | 1.58 | 53.71 | 56.87 | ||
5岁 | 男 | (16, 16) | 68.75 | 2.76 | 65.99 | 71.51 | 64.81 | 3.53 | 61.28 | 68.34 | |
女 | (15, 12) | 66.75 | 3.07 | 63.68 | 69.82 | 64.59 | 3.29 | 61.30 | 67.88 | ||
6岁 | 男 | (15, 14) | 77.71 | 1.78 | 75.93 | 79.49 | 78.6 | 1.49 | 77.11 | 80.09 | |
女 | (12, 15) | 77.38 | 2.15 | 75.23 | 79.53 | 77.8 | 1.79 | 76.01 | 79.59 |
表5幼儿在不同信息正确性条件下对高面孔吸引力者的询问率和赞同率(M ± SD)
年龄组(n1, n2) | 正确性为50% vs. 50% | 正确性为25% vs. 75% | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
询问率 | 赞同率 | 询问率 | 赞同率 | |
4岁(34, 28) | 0.61 ± 0.29 | 0.55 ± 0.21 | 0.66 ± 0.22 | 0.31 ± 0.33 |
5岁(31, 28) | 0.71 ± 0.33 | 0.56 ± 0.27 | 0.72 ± 0.26 | 0.24 ± 0.23 |
6岁(27, 29) | 0.80 ± 0.22 | 0.58 ± 0.24 | 0.70 ± 0.26 | 0.17 ± 0.17 |
表5幼儿在不同信息正确性条件下对高面孔吸引力者的询问率和赞同率(M ± SD)
年龄组(n1, n2) | 正确性为50% vs. 50% | 正确性为25% vs. 75% | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
询问率 | 赞同率 | 询问率 | 赞同率 | |
4岁(34, 28) | 0.61 ± 0.29 | 0.55 ± 0.21 | 0.66 ± 0.22 | 0.31 ± 0.33 |
5岁(31, 28) | 0.71 ± 0.33 | 0.56 ± 0.27 | 0.72 ± 0.26 | 0.24 ± 0.23 |
6岁(27, 29) | 0.80 ± 0.22 | 0.58 ± 0.24 | 0.70 ± 0.26 | 0.17 ± 0.17 |
表6幼儿在不同信息正确性条件下的选择性信任
年龄组(n1, n2) | 正确性为50% vs. 50% | 正确性为25% vs. 75% | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
询问率 | 赞同率 | 询问率 | 赞同率 | |||||
t | d | t | d | t | d | t | d | |
4岁(34, 28) | 2.24* | 2.10 | 1.44 | 3.87** | 0.73 | -3.10** | 0.58 | |
5岁(31, 28) | 3.70** | 0.64 | 1.26 | 4.46*** | 0.85 | -5.83*** | 1.13 | |
6岁(27, 29) | 7.03*** | 1.36 | 1.70 | 4.22*** | 0.77 | -10.62*** | 1.94 |
表6幼儿在不同信息正确性条件下的选择性信任
年龄组(n1, n2) | 正确性为50% vs. 50% | 正确性为25% vs. 75% | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
询问率 | 赞同率 | 询问率 | 赞同率 | |||||
t | d | t | d | t | d | t | d | |
4岁(34, 28) | 2.24* | 2.10 | 1.44 | 3.87** | 0.73 | -3.10** | 0.58 | |
5岁(31, 28) | 3.70** | 0.64 | 1.26 | 4.46*** | 0.85 | -5.83*** | 1.13 | |
6岁(27, 29) | 7.03*** | 1.36 | 1.70 | 4.22*** | 0.77 | -10.62*** | 1.94 |
表7幼儿对高面孔吸引力者选择性信任程度
年龄组 | 正确性为50% vs. 50% | 正确性为25% vs. 75% | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | M | SD | n | M | SD | |
4岁 | 34 | 0.11 | 0.29 | 28 | 0.16 | 0.22 |
5岁 | 31 | 0.22 | 0.33 | 28 | 0.22 | 0.26 |
6岁 | 27 | 0.21 | 0.29 | 29 | 0.20 | 0.26 |
表7幼儿对高面孔吸引力者选择性信任程度
年龄组 | 正确性为50% vs. 50% | 正确性为25% vs. 75% | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | M | SD | n | M | SD | |
4岁 | 34 | 0.11 | 0.29 | 28 | 0.16 | 0.22 |
5岁 | 31 | 0.22 | 0.33 | 28 | 0.22 | 0.26 |
6岁 | 27 | 0.21 | 0.29 | 29 | 0.20 | 0.26 |
图2幼儿基于面孔的选择性信任心理加工过程示意图
图2幼儿基于面孔的选择性信任心理加工过程示意图
参考文献 64
[1] | Bascandziev I., & Harris P.L. ( 2014). In beauty we trust: Children prefer information from more attractive informants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32( 1), 94-99. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12022URLpmid: 24164592 |
[2] | Bascandziev I., & Harris P.L. ( 2016). The beautiful and the accurate: Are children’s selective trust decisions biased?. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 92-105. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.017URLpmid: 27518811 |
[3] | Baumeister R. F., Bratslavsky E., Finkenauer C., & Vohs K. D . ( 2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5( 4), 323-370. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323URL |
[4] | Bernard S., Harris P., Terrier N., & Clément F . ( 2015). Children weigh the number of informants and perceptual uncertainty when identifying objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 136, 70-81. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.03.009URLpmid: 25872680 |
[5] | Birch S. A. J., Vauthier S. A., & Bloom P . ( 2008). Three-and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning. Cognition, 107( 3), 1018-1034. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.008URLpmid: 18295193 |
[6] | Boothroyd L. G., Meins E., Vukovic J., & Burt D. M . ( 2014). Developmental changes in children's facial preferences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35( 5), 376-383. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.05.002URL |
[7] | Cao M-L., Gong W-J., & Tang Y-L . ( 2012). Impact of bystander’s non-verbal cues on preschoolers’ selective trust. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20( 5), 613-615. |
[ 曹梦露, 龚文进, 汤永隆 . ( 2012). 旁观者非言语线索对幼儿选择性信任的影响. 中国临床心理学杂志, 20( 5), 613-615.] | |
[8] | Chan C. C.Y., & Tardif T. ,( 2013). Knowing better: The role of prior knowledge and culture in trust in testimony. Developmental Psychology, 49( 3), 591-601. doi: 10.1037/a0031336URLpmid: 23294151 |
[9] | Che H.B . ( 2014). The developmental characteristics of 4-to 5-year-old children’s selective trust judgment based on pro-social traits (Unpublished master’s thesis). Liaoning Normal University, China. |
[ 车翰博 . ( 2014). 基于亲社会特质的4-5岁幼儿选择性信任判断的发展特点(硕士论文). 辽宁师范大学.] | |
[10] | Clément F., Koenig M., & Harris P . ( 2004). The ontogenesis of trust. Mind and Language, 19( 4), 360-379. |
[11] | Cong H. Q.& Xu X. J. ( 2003). How was Testimony defended. Philosophical Researches( 10), 67-76. |
[ 丛杭青, 徐献军 . ( 2003). Testimony是如何得到辩护的. 哲学研究 ( 10), 69-76.] | |
[12] | Corriveau K. H., Harris P. L., Meins E., Fernyhough C., Arnott B., Elliott L., .. De Rosnay M . ( 2009). Young children’s trust in their mother’s claims: Longitudinal links with attachment security in infancy. Child Development, 80( 3), 750-761. |
[13] | Corriveau K. H., Kinzler K. D., & Harris P. L . ( 2012). Accuracy trumps accent in children's endorsement of object labels. Developmental Psychology, 49( 3), 470-479. doi: 10.1037/a0030604URLpmid: 23231692 |
[14] | Ding X.C . ( 2016). Preschoolers’ selective trust in informants (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). East China Normal University, Shang Hai. |
[ 丁雪辰 . ( 2016). 幼儿对信息提供者的选择性信任研究(博士学位论文). 华东师范大学, 上海.] | |
[15] | Dion K.K . ( 1973). Young children's stereotyping of facial attractiveness. Developmental Psychology, 9( 2), 183-188. doi: 10.1037/h0035083URL |
[16] | Doebel S., & Koenig M.A. ( 2013). Children's use of moral behavior in selective trust: Discrimination versus learning. Developmental Psychology, 49( 3), 462-469. doi: 10.1037/a0031595URLpmid: 23356525 |
[17] | Eagly A. H., Ashmore R. D., Makhijani M. G., & Longo L. C . ( 1991). What is beautiful is good, but…: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110( 1), 109-128. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.109URL |
[18] | Ellsberg D.., ( 1961). Risk, ambiguity, and the savage axioms. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 75( 4), 643-669. doi: 10.2307/1884324URL |
[19] | Feingold A.., ( 1992). Good-looking people are not what we think. Psychological Bulletin, 111( 2), 304-341. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.304URL |
[20] | Harris P. L., Corriveau K. H., Pasquini E. S., Koenig M., Fusaro M., & Clément F . ( 2012). Credulity and the development of selective trust in early childhood. Foundations of Metacognition, 193-210. |
[21] | Hertz-Lazarowitz R., Feitelson D., Zahavi S., & Hartup W. W . ( 1981). Social interaction and social organization of Israeli five to seven-year-olds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4( 2), 143-155. doi: 10.1177/016502548100400201URL |
[22] | Horst J.S., & Hout M.C . ( 2016). The novel object and unusual name (NOUN) database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research. Behavior Research Methods. 48( 4), 1393-1409. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0647-3URLpmid: 26424438 |
[23] | Jackson L. A., Hunter J. E., & Hodge C. N . ( 1995). Physical attractiveness and intellectual competence: A meta-analytic review. Social Psychology Quarterly, 58( 2), 108-122. doi: 10.2307/2787149URL |
[24] | Jaffer S.& Ma L. , ( 2015). Preschoolers show less trust in physically disabled or obese informants. Frontiers in Psychology, 5( 10), 1524. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01524URLpmid: 25610413 |
[25] | Jaswal V.K . ( 2004). Don't believe everything you hear: Preschoolers' sensitivity to speaker intent in category induction. Child Development, 75( 6), 1871-1885. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00822.xURLpmid: 15566385 |
[26] | Jaswal V.K . ( 2006). Preschoolers favor the creator's label when reasoning about an artifact's function. Cognition, 99( 3), B83-B92. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.07.006URLpmid: 16185679 |
[27] | Jaswal V.K., & Markman E.M . ( 2007). Looks aren't everything: 24-month-olds' willingness to accept unexpected labels. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8( 1), 93-111. doi: 10.1080/15248370709336995URL |
[28] | Jaswal V.K.&, Neely L.A, 2006). Adults don't always know best preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science, 17( 9), 757-758. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.xURL |
[29] | Kinzler K. D., Corriveau K. H., & Harris P. L . ( 2011). Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science, 14( 1), 106-111. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00965.xURL |
[30] | Koenig M. A., Clément F., & Harris P. L . ( 2004). Trust in testimony: Children's use of true and false statements. Psychological Science, 15( 10), 694-698. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.xURLpmid: 15447641 |
[31] | Koenig M.A.&, Harris P.L, 2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76( 6), 1261-1277. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.xURLpmid: 16274439 |
[32] | Koenig M.A.&, Jaswal V.K, 2011). Characterizing children’s expectations about expertise and incompetence: Halo or pitchfork effects? Child Development, 82( 5), 1634-1647. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01618.xURL |
[33] | Koenig M.A.&, Woodward A.L, 2010). Sensitivity of 24- month-olds to the prior inaccuracy of the source: Possible mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 46( 4), 815-26. doi: 10.1037/a0019664URLpmid: 20604604 |
[34] | Kushnir T., Vredenburgh C., & Schneider L. A . ( 2013). "Who can help me fix this toy?" The distinction between causal knowledge and word knowledge guides preschoolers' selective requests for information. Developmental Psychology, 49( 3), 446-453. doi: 10.1037/a0031649URLpmid: 23339590 |
[35] | Langlois J. H., Kalakanis L., Rubenstein A. J., Larson A., Hallam M., & Smoot M . ( 2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126( 3), 390-423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390URL |
[36] | Li D.Ed). ( 1987) . Childhood developmental psychology. Shanghai: East China Normal University Press. |
[ 李丹 . (主编).( 1987). 儿童发展心理学. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.] | |
[37] | Li Q-G., Heyman G. D., Xu F., & Lee K . ( 2014). Young children’s use of honesty as a basis for selective trust. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117, 59-72. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.09.002URLpmid: 24149377 |
[38] | Liu D., Vanderbilt K. E., & Heyman G. D . ( 2013). Selective trust: Children's use of intention and outcome of past testimony. Developmental Psychology, 49( 3), 439-445. doi: 10.1037/a0031615URLpmid: 23339589 |
[39] | Liu J.M., & Yin G. E . ( 2001). Review on the development of children's classification. Psychological Science, 24( 6), 707-709. |
[ 刘金明, 阴国恩 . ( 2001). 儿童分类发展研究综述. 心理科学, 24( 6), 707-709.] doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1671-6981.2001.06.018URL | |
[40] | Ma L., & Ganea P.A. ( 2010). Dealing with conflicting information: Young children's reliance on what they see versus what they are told. Developmental Science, 13( 1), 151-160. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00878.xURLpmid: 20121871 |
[41] | Ma L., & Woolley J.D. ( 2013). Young children's sensitivity to speaker gender when learning from others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14( 1), 100-119. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.638687URL |
[42] | Mascaro O.& Sperber D. ,( 2009). The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children’s vigilance towards deception. Cognition, 112( 3), 367-380. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.012URL |
[43] | Miller C. F., Lurye L. E., Zosuls K. M., & Ruble D. N . ( 2009). Accessibility of gender stereotype domains: Developmental and gender differences in children. Sex Roles, 60( 11-12), 870-881. doi: 10.1007/s11199-009-9584-xURLpmid: 2709873 |
[44] | Nurmsoo E., & Robinson E.J. ( 2009). Identifying unreliable informants: Do children excuse past inaccuracy?. Developmental Science, 12( 1), 41-47. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00750.xURLpmid: 19120411 |
[45] | Oosterhof N.N., & Todorov A. , ( 2009). Shared perceptual basis of emotional expressions and trustworthiness impressions from faces. Emotion, 9( 1), 128-133. doi: 10.1037/a0014520URLpmid: 19186926 |
[46] | Pham M.T., & Avnet T. , ( 2009). Contingent reliance on the affect heuristic as a function of regulatory focus. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108( 2), 267-278. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.10.001URL |
[47] | Rennels J.L.&, Langlois J.H, 2014 a). Children's attractiveness, gender, and race biases: A comparison of their strength and generality. Child Development, 85( 4), 1401-1418. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12226URLpmid: 4107158 |
[48] | Rennels J.L.&, Langlois J.H, 2014 b). Children’s classification and lexicalization of attractiveness, gender, and race: Differential displays of these concepts and relatedness to bias and flexibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126( 6), 1-18. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.009URL |
[49] | Robinson E. J., Butterfill S. A., & Nurmsoo E . ( 2011). Gaining knowledge via other minds: Children's flexible trust in others as sources of information. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29( 4), 961-980. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02036.xURLpmid: 21995747 |
[50] | Ronfard S., & Lane J.D. ( 2017). Preschoolers continually adjust their epistemic trust based on an informant's ongoing accuracy. Child Development, 89( 2), 414-429. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12720URLpmid: 28105637 |
[51] | Rozin P., & Royzman E.B. ( 2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5( 4), 296-320. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2URL |
[52] | Sabbagh M.A.&, Baldwin D.A, 2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers' theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72( 4), 1054-1070. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00334URLpmid: 11480934 |
[53] | Sobel D.M.&, Corriveau K.H, 2010). Children monitor individuals' expertise for word learning. Child Development, 81( 2), 669-679. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01422.xURLpmid: 20438467 |
[54] | Sommer W., Hildebrandt A., Kunina-Habenicht O., Schacht A., & Wilhelm O . ( 2013). Sex differences in face cognition. Acta Psychologica, 142( 1), 62-73. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.001URLpmid: 23232336 |
[55] | Sperber D., Clément F., Heintz C., Mascaro O., Mercier H., Origgi G., & Wilson D . ( 2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25( 4), 359-393. |
[56] | TaylorM. M.G . ( 2013). Gender influences on children’s selective trust of adult testimony. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115( 4), 672-690. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.003URLpmid: 23708732 |
[57] | Todorov A. ( 2008). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness: An extension of systems for recognition of emotions signaling approach/avoidance behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124( 1), 208-224. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.012URL |
[58] | Todorov A., Pakrashi M., & Oosterhof N. N . ( 2009). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness after minimal time exposure. Social Cognition, 27( 6), 813-833. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.813URL |
[59] | Vanderbilt K. E., Liu D., & Heyman G. D . ( 2011). The development of distrust. Child Development, 82( 5), 1372-1380. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01629.xURL |
[60] | Wang Y. ( 2012). The developmental mechanism of 3 to 5 years old children’s selective trust ability. ( Unpublished Master degree thesis). Shenyang Normal University. |
[ 王颖 . ( 2012). 3-5岁幼儿选择性信任能力的发展机制(硕士论文). 沈阳师范大学.] | |
[61] | Wang Y. Q., Yao P. F., & Zhou G. M . ( 2015). The influence of facial attractiveness and personality labels on men and women’s mate preference. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47( 1), 108-118. |
[ 王雨晴, 姚鹏飞, 周国梅 . ( 2015). 面孔吸引力、人格标签对于男女择偶偏好的影响. 心理学报, 47( 1), 108-118.] doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00108URL | |
[62] | Yang L., & Hu Z.Q . ( 2010). Affect Heuristic in Bayesian Reasoning. Psychological Exploration, 30( 3), 18-21. |
[ 杨莉, 胡竹菁 . ( 2010). 贝叶斯推理中的情感启发式. 心理学探新, 30( 3), 18-21.] doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1003-5184.2010.03.005URL | |
[63] | Yuan M., Deng Z., & Ji P . ( 2013). Children’s social epistemology: Children’s selective trust in informants. Advances in Psychological Science, 21( 3), 480-486. |
[ 袁鸣, 邓铸, 季培 . ( 2013). 儿童社会认识论: 儿童对信息提供者的选择性信任. 心理科学进展, 21( 3), 480-486.] doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00480URL | |
[64] | Zhang L.P.& Zhou H. ,( 2011). Children’s selective trust based on judgments of information: Its relationship with prosocial behavior and decision making. Psychological Development and Education, 27( 1), 71-75. |
[ 张兰萍, 周晖 . ( 2011). 幼儿基于信息判断的选择性信任——与亲社会行为及决策判断的关系. 心理发展与教育, 27( 1), 71-75.] |
相关文章 15
[1] | 田欣然, 侯文霞, 欧玉晓, 易冰, 陈文锋, 尚俊辰. 基于合成平均刺激的平均表征机制——来自平均面孔吸引力的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 714-728. |
[2] | 姚尧, 陈晓湘. 音乐训练对4~5岁幼儿普通话声调范畴感知能力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(4): 456-468. |
[3] | 贺晓玲, 陈俊. 3~5岁幼儿权力概念多重隐喻的认知发展[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(2): 149-161. |
[4] | 金心怡,周冰欣,孟斐. 3岁幼儿的二级观点采择及合作互动的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(9): 1028-1039. |
[5] | 李泉,宋亚男,廉彬,冯廷勇. 正念训练提升3~4岁幼儿注意力和执行功能[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(3): 324-336. |
[6] | 韩尚锋, 李 玥, 刘燊, 徐强, 谭群, 张林. 美在观察者眼中:陌生面孔吸引力评价中 的晕轮效应与泛化效应[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(4): 363-376. |
[7] | 李婷玉, 刘黎, 李宜霖, 朱莉琪. 冲突情境下幼儿的选择性信任和信念修正[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(12): 1390-1399. |
[8] | 李婷玉;刘黎;朱莉琪. 4~6岁幼儿经济博弈中的信任行为及其影响因素[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(1): 17-27. |
[9] | 胡清芬;卢静. 幼儿在地图任务中对自我位置与自我朝向的结合[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(9): 1143-1150. |
[10] | 邢淑芬;梁熙;岳建宏;王争艳. 祖辈共同养育背景下多重依恋关系及对幼儿社会−情绪性发展的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(5): 518-528. |
[11] | 钱淼;周立霞;鲁甜甜;翁梦星;傅根跃. 幼儿友好型内隐联想测验的建构及有效性[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(7): 903-913. |
[12] | 王福兴;李文静;颜志强;段朝辉;李卉. 幼儿对威胁性刺激蛇的注意觉察:来自眼动证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(6): 774-786. |
[13] | 卢静;胡清芬. 幼儿在地图任务中自我位置与自我朝向表征能力的发展[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(2): 234-242. |
[14] | 王雨晴;姚鹏飞;周国梅. 面孔吸引力、人格标签对于男女择偶偏好的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(1): 108-118. |
[15] | 贺琼;王争艳;王莉;蒋彩虹;上官芳芳. 新入园幼儿的皮质醇变化与上呼吸道感染的关系:气质的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(4): 516-527. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4361