删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

自我信息识别优势——来自注意定向网络的证据

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

高虹1, 李杨卓1, 胡蝶1, 朱敏2,3, 高湘萍1, 胡天翊1()
1 上海师范大学教育学院心理系, 上海200234
2 南京工业大学社会工作与管理系, 南京 211816
3 南京工业大学社会创新与发展研究所, 南京 211816
收稿日期:2017-10-17出版日期:2018-11-30发布日期:2018-10-30




Cognitive advantage for self-information: Evidence from the orienting network of attention

GAO Hong1, LI Yangzhuo1, HU Die1, ZHU Min2,3, GAO Xiangping1, HU Tianyi1()
1 Department of Psychology, School of Education, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai 200234, China
2 Department of Social Work and Management, Nanjing Tech University, Nanjing 211816, China
3 Institute of Social Innovation and Development, Nanjing Tech University, Nanjing 211816, China
Received:2017-10-17Online:2018-11-30Published:2018-10-30







摘要/Abstract


摘要: 研究采用注意网络测验任务(attention network test, ANT), 分别检验自我相关信息在注意的警觉、定向和执行控制网络上的加工效率差异, 以揭示自我信息识别优势的注意机制。实验1考察了面孔类型(自我面孔或他人面孔)在3种注意网络下的加工效率差异; 实验2采用自我联结学习范式对实验1进行验证; 实验3将颜色类型(红色或绿色)作为目标刺激, 面孔类型作为背景, 以考察任务无关自我信息是否对注意网络加工效率有影响。研究结果发现, 当自我信息为目标时, 个体在注意定向网络上存在加工效率优势, 而警觉和执行控制网络上没有加工效率差异。当自我相关信息与任务无关时, 在注意的警觉、定向和执行控制网络中均未表现出加工效率优势。说明了自我信息的注意优势发生在注意定向网络上, 且受任务优先性影响。



图1注意网络测验单个试次示意图
图1注意网络测验单个试次示意图


表1实验1各条件下被试识别面孔的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 线索类型 自我面孔 朋友面孔
反应时 正确率 反应时 正确率
侧翼一致 无线索 684 ± 59 97.32 ± 3.96 714 ± 65 97.32 ± 6.02
双重线索 636 ± 60 97.62 ± 4.45 677 ± 63 98.21 ± 4.16
中央线索 640 ± 56 96.13 ± 5.77 683 ± 59 98.51 ± 3.25
空间线索 587 ± 62 97.92 ± 4.32 633 ± 61 97.62 ± 4.45
平均值 636 ± 53 97.25 ± 2.60 676 ± 54 97.92 ± 2.78
侧翼冲突 无线索 698 ± 58 91.13 ± 6.20 723 ± 45 97.02 ± 5.18
双重线索 654 ± 65 97.02 ± 4.66 686 ± 60 97.62 ± 4.45
中央线索 670 ± 63 94.94 ± 7.64 698 ± 65 95.83 ± 6.99
空间线索 596 ± 73 98.21 ± 4.12 653 ± 65 98.21 ± 4.16
平均值 654 ± 56 96.80 ± 3.08 690 ± 51 97.17 ± 3.22

表1实验1各条件下被试识别面孔的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 线索类型 自我面孔 朋友面孔
反应时 正确率 反应时 正确率
侧翼一致 无线索 684 ± 59 97.32 ± 3.96 714 ± 65 97.32 ± 6.02
双重线索 636 ± 60 97.62 ± 4.45 677 ± 63 98.21 ± 4.16
中央线索 640 ± 56 96.13 ± 5.77 683 ± 59 98.51 ± 3.25
空间线索 587 ± 62 97.92 ± 4.32 633 ± 61 97.62 ± 4.45
平均值 636 ± 53 97.25 ± 2.60 676 ± 54 97.92 ± 2.78
侧翼冲突 无线索 698 ± 58 91.13 ± 6.20 723 ± 45 97.02 ± 5.18
双重线索 654 ± 65 97.02 ± 4.66 686 ± 60 97.62 ± 4.45
中央线索 670 ± 63 94.94 ± 7.64 698 ± 65 95.83 ± 6.99
空间线索 596 ± 73 98.21 ± 4.12 653 ± 65 98.21 ± 4.16
平均值 654 ± 56 96.80 ± 3.08 690 ± 51 97.17 ± 3.22



图2实验1中自我和朋友面孔在注意网络的加工效率注: 两种面孔在注意网络的平均反应时, *p = 0.02误差线: 95%可信区间
图2实验1中自我和朋友面孔在注意网络的加工效率注: 两种面孔在注意网络的平均反应时, *p = 0.02误差线: 95%可信区间



图3图形-身份标签联结学习任务流程图(a)及ANT目标屏示例(b)
图3图形-身份标签联结学习任务流程图(a)及ANT目标屏示例(b)


表2加工不同身份图形的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 线索类型 自我图形 朋友图形 无社会意义图形
反应时 正确率 反应时 正确率 反应时 正确率
侧翼一致 无线索 613 ± 64 98.33 ± 3.39 627 ± 58 97.50 ± 4.97 647 ± 80 97.22 ± 5.03
双重线索 553 ± 64 98.06 ± 3.58 562 ± 62 97.22 ± 4.55 581 ± 76 96.39 ± 5.66
中央线索 563 ± 62 96.39 ± 4.20 566 ± 67 96.67 ± 4.69 570 ± 76 97.78 ± 6.16
空间线索 526 ± 62 96.67 ± 6.03 549 ± 62 97.50 ± 4.46 572 ± 81 96.95 ± 4.63
平均值 564 ± 58 97.36 ± 3.32 576 ± 59 97.22 ± 3.30 593 ± 71 97.08 ± 4.53
侧翼冲突 无线索 635 ± 71 96.11 ± 6.09 634 ± 58 98.06 ± 4.20 649 ± 68 98.06 ± 3.58
双重线索 574 ± 62 98.33 ± 3.39 587 ± 68 96.94 ± 5.99 602 ± 68 98.61 ± 3.84
中央线索 587 ± 54 98.06 ± 4.20 584 ± 56 97.22 ± 5.93 608 ± 72 97.78 ± 4.86
空间线索 550 ± 59 97.78 ± 4.86 575 ± 61 98.61 ± 3.84 585 ± 68 97.22 ± 5.05
平均值 586 ± 57 97.57 ± 3.33 595 ± 56 97.71 ± 3.60 611 ± 65 97.92 ± 3.37

表2加工不同身份图形的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 线索类型 自我图形 朋友图形 无社会意义图形
反应时 正确率 反应时 正确率 反应时 正确率
侧翼一致 无线索 613 ± 64 98.33 ± 3.39 627 ± 58 97.50 ± 4.97 647 ± 80 97.22 ± 5.03
双重线索 553 ± 64 98.06 ± 3.58 562 ± 62 97.22 ± 4.55 581 ± 76 96.39 ± 5.66
中央线索 563 ± 62 96.39 ± 4.20 566 ± 67 96.67 ± 4.69 570 ± 76 97.78 ± 6.16
空间线索 526 ± 62 96.67 ± 6.03 549 ± 62 97.50 ± 4.46 572 ± 81 96.95 ± 4.63
平均值 564 ± 58 97.36 ± 3.32 576 ± 59 97.22 ± 3.30 593 ± 71 97.08 ± 4.53
侧翼冲突 无线索 635 ± 71 96.11 ± 6.09 634 ± 58 98.06 ± 4.20 649 ± 68 98.06 ± 3.58
双重线索 574 ± 62 98.33 ± 3.39 587 ± 68 96.94 ± 5.99 602 ± 68 98.61 ± 3.84
中央线索 587 ± 54 98.06 ± 4.20 584 ± 56 97.22 ± 5.93 608 ± 72 97.78 ± 4.86
空间线索 550 ± 59 97.78 ± 4.86 575 ± 61 98.61 ± 3.84 585 ± 68 97.22 ± 5.05
平均值 586 ± 57 97.57 ± 3.33 595 ± 56 97.71 ± 3.60 611 ± 65 97.92 ± 3.37



图4不同意义图形的注意网络加工效率注:三种不同意义图形在注意网络的平均反应时, *p < 0.001误差线:95%可信区间
图4不同意义图形的注意网络加工效率注:三种不同意义图形在注意网络的平均反应时, *p < 0.001误差线:95%可信区间



图5实验3中目标及侧翼冲突示例注:彩图见电子版
图5实验3中目标及侧翼冲突示例注:彩图见电子版


表3实验3各条件下被试识别颜色的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 线索类型 红色 绿色
反应时 正确率 反应时 正确率
侧翼一致 无线索 664 ± 114 97.92 ± 3.68 674 ± 116 98.26 ± 5.48
双重线索 683 ± 104 98.61 ± 3.17 693 ± 116 98.61 ± 4.01
中央线索 632 ± 117 96.88 ± 5.39 643 ± 123 98.96 ± 2.81
空间线索 695 ± 114 98.96 ± 2.81 702 ± 110 98.61 ±3.17
平均值 678 ± 111 97.66 ± 2.07 669 ± 105 98.35 ± 2.30
侧翼冲突 无线索 656 ± 119 96.53 ± 6.46 678 ± 117 97.92 ± 4.43
双重线索 614 ± 123 97.57 ± 4.58 628 ± 142 98.61 ± 3.17
中央线索 640 ± 122 96.18 ± 5.48 637 ± 125 97.22 ± 5.31
空间线索 630 ± 116 98.96 ± 2.81 650 ± 133 98.96 ± 2.81
平均值 648 ± 124 97.31 ± 2.34 635 ± 116 97.74 ± 2.67

表3实验3各条件下被试识别颜色的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 线索类型 红色 绿色
反应时 正确率 反应时 正确率
侧翼一致 无线索 664 ± 114 97.92 ± 3.68 674 ± 116 98.26 ± 5.48
双重线索 683 ± 104 98.61 ± 3.17 693 ± 116 98.61 ± 4.01
中央线索 632 ± 117 96.88 ± 5.39 643 ± 123 98.96 ± 2.81
空间线索 695 ± 114 98.96 ± 2.81 702 ± 110 98.61 ±3.17
平均值 678 ± 111 97.66 ± 2.07 669 ± 105 98.35 ± 2.30
侧翼冲突 无线索 656 ± 119 96.53 ± 6.46 678 ± 117 97.92 ± 4.43
双重线索 614 ± 123 97.57 ± 4.58 628 ± 142 98.61 ± 3.17
中央线索 640 ± 122 96.18 ± 5.48 637 ± 125 97.22 ± 5.31
空间线索 630 ± 116 98.96 ± 2.81 650 ± 133 98.96 ± 2.81
平均值 648 ± 124 97.31 ± 2.34 635 ± 116 97.74 ± 2.67



图6自我和朋友面孔注意网络的加工效率注:误差线:95%可信区间
图6自我和朋友面孔注意网络的加工效率注:误差线:95%可信区间







1 Callejas A., Lupià?ez J., Funes M. J., & Tudela P . ( 2005). Modulations among the alerting, orienting and executive control networks. Experimental Brain Research, 167( 1), 27-37.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2365-zURLpmid: 16021429
2 Corbetta M., &Shulman , G. L . ( 2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3( 3), 201-215.
3 Corbetta M., Patel G., & Shulman G. L . ( 2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58( 3), 306-324.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017URLpmid: 18466742
4 Crottaz-Herbette S., &Menon V. , ( 2006). Where and when the anterior cingulate cortex modulates attentional response: Combined fMRI and ERP evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18( 5), 766-780.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.5.766URLpmid: 16768376
5 Devue C., &Brédart S. , ( 2008). Attention to self-referential stimuli: Can I ignore my own face? Acta Psychologica, 128( 2), 290-297.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.004URLpmid: 18413272
6 Devue C., Laloyaux C., Feyers D., Theeuwes J., & Brédart S . ( 2009). Do pictures of faces, and which ones, capture attention in the inattentional-blindness paradigm? Perception, 38( 4), 552-568.
7 Devue C., Van der Stigchel S., Brédart S., & Theeuwes J . ( 2009). You do not find your own face faster; you just look at it longer. Cognition, 111( 1), 114-122.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.003URLpmid: 19230872
8 Engle R.W., &Kane , M. J . ( 2003). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 145-199.
9 Fan J., McCandliss B. D., Fossella J., Flombaum J. I., & Posner M. I . ( 2005). The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26( 2), 471-479.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.004URLpmid: 15907304
10 Fan J., McCandliss B. D., Sommer T., Raz A., & Posner M. I . ( 2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14( 3), 340-347.
doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886URLpmid: 11970796
11 Farrant K., &Uddin , L. Q . ( 2015). Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention networks in the human brain. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 165-174.
doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001URLpmid: 4396619
12 Federico F., Marotta A., Adriani T., Maccari L., & Casagrande M . ( 2013). Attention network test — The impact of social information on executive control, alerting and orienting. Acta Psychologica, 143( 1), 65-70.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.02.006URLpmid: 23542806
13 Gao W., Gilmore J. H., Shen D., Smith J. K., Zhu H., & Lin W . ( 2013). The synchronization within and interaction between the default and dorsal attention networks in early infancy. Cerebral Cortex, 23( 3), 594-603.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs043URLpmid: 3563337
14 Garza J. P., Strom M. J., Wright C. E., Roberts R. J., & Reed C. L . ( 2013). Top-down influences mediate hand bias in spatial attention. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75( 5), 819-823.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0480-7URLpmid: 23722884
15 Hackley S.A., &Valle-Inclán F. , ( 1999). Accessory stimulus effects on response selection: Does arousal speed decision making? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11( 3), 321-329.
doi: 10.1162/089892999563427URLpmid: 10402259
16 Hackley S.A., &Valle-Inclán F. , ( 2003). Which stages of processing are speeded by a warning signal? Biological Psychology, 64( 1-2), 27-45.
doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00101-7URLpmid: 14602354
17 Haykin S., Fatemi M., Setoodeh P., & Xue Y . ( 2012). Cognitive control. Proceedings of the IEEE, 100( 12), 3156-3169.
18 Indovina I. &Macaluso E. , ( 2007). Dissociation of stimulus relevance and saliency factors during shifts of visuospatial attention. Cerebral Cortex, 17( 7), 1701-1711.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl081URLpmid: 17003078
19 Jannati A., Gaspar J. M., & McDonald J. J . ( 2013). Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39( 6), 1713-1730.
doi: 10.1037/a0032251URLpmid: 23527999
20 Keyes H. &Dlugokencka A. , ( 2014). Do I have my attention? Speed of processing advantages for the self-face are not driven by automatic attention capture. PLoS One, 9( 10), e110792.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110792URLpmid: 4206440
21 Kim J., Kang M. S., Cho Y. S., & Lee S. H . ( 2017). Prolonged interruption of cognitive control of conflict processing over human faces by task-irrelevant emotion expression. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1024.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01024URLpmid: 5476788
22 Kratz O., Studer P., Malcherek S., Erbe K., Moll G. H., & Heinrich H . ( 2011). Attentional processes in children with ADHD: An event-related potential study using the attention network test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 81( 2), 82-90.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.05.008URLpmid: 21641942
23 Li H., &Cai H.D, . ( 2013). The modulation of emotion on the attentional function networks. Advances in Psychological Science, 21( 1), 59-67.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00059URL
24 [ 李贺, 蔡厚德 . ( 2013). 情绪对注意功能网络的调制. 心理科学进展, 21( 1), 59-67.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00059URL
25 Liu M. H., Wang L. Y., Sui J., & Zhang M . ( 2012). Modulation of self-face for visual spatial attention: Evidence from a Posner’s cueing paradigm. Journal of Psychological Science, 35( 1), 24-29.
26 Mackie M. A., Dam N. T. V., & Fan J . ( 2013). Cognitive control and attentional functions. Brain and Cognition, 82( 3), 301-312.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.004URLpmid: 23792472
27 Moray N.., ( 1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11( 1), 56-60.
doi: 10.1080/17470215908416289URL
28 Mueller. C.J., &Kuchinke , L. ( 2016). Processing of face identity in the affective flanker task: A diffusion model analysis. Psychological Research, 80( 6), 1-11.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0696-3URLpmid: 26253324
29 Neuhaus A. H., Urbanek C., Opgen-Rhein C., Hahn E., Ta T. M. T., Koehler S., .. Dettling M . ( 2010). Event-related potentials associated with attention network test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 76( 2), 72-79.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.02.005URLpmid: 20184924
30 Ninomiya H., Onitsuka T., Chen C. H., Sato E., & Tashiro N . ( 1998). P300 in response to the subject’s own face. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 52( 5), 519-522.
doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.1998.00445.xURLpmid: 10215014
31 Palermo R., & Rhodes G . ( 2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia, 45( 1), 75-92.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.025URLpmid: 16797607
32 Pannese A., &Hirsch J. , ( 2010). Self-specific priming effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 19( 4), 962-968.
33 Petersen, S. E., &Posner , M. I . ( 2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35( 5), 73-89.
34 Posner, M. I., &Petersen , S. E . ( 1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42.
35 Rothbart, M. K., &Posner , M. I . ( 2015). The developing brain in a multitasking world. Developmental Review, 35, 42-63.
doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.006URLpmid: 25821335
36 Shulman G. L., D'Avossa G., Tansy A. P., & Corbetta M . ( 2002). Two attentional processes in the parietal lobe. Cerebral Cortex, 12( 11), 1124-1131.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.11.1124URLpmid: 12379601
37 Spagna A., Martella D., Sebastiani M., Maccari L., Marotta A., & Casagrande M . ( 2014). Efficiency and interactions of alerting, orienting and executive networks: The impact of imperative stimulus type. Acta Psychologica, 148( 3), 209-215.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.007URLpmid: 24607440
38 Sui J., Liu C. H., Wang L., & Han S . ( 2009). Attentional orientation induced by temporarily established self- referential cues. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62( 5), 844-849.
doi: 10.1080/17470210802559393URLpmid: 19132633
39 Sui J., Rotshtein P., & Humphreys G. W . ( 2013). Coupling social attention to the self forms a network for personal significance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110( 19), 7607-7612.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221862110URL
40 Sui J., Zhu Y., & Han S. H . ( 2006). Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: An event-related brain potential study. Neuroreport, 17( 4), 423-427.
doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000203357.65190.61URLpmid: 16514370
41 Tang X., Wu J., & Shen Y . ( 2006). The interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and exogenous attention. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 61( 14), 208-224
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.002URLpmid: 26546734
42 Tong F.., &Nakayama , K. ( 1999). Robust representations for faces: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25( 4), 1016-1035.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.25.4.1016URLpmid: 10464943
43 Turk D. J., Brady-van Den Bos M., Collard P., Gillespie- Smith K., Conway M. A., & Cunningham S. J . ( 2013). Divided attention selectively impairs memory for self- relevant information. Memory & Cognition, 41( 4), 503-510.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0279-0URLpmid: 23263878
44 Uddin L.Q . ( 2015). Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16( 1), 55-61.
doi: 10.1038/nrn3857URLpmid: 25406711
45 Visintin E., De Panfilis C., Antonucci C., Capecci C., Marchesi C., & Sambataro F . ( 2015). Parsing the intrinsic networks underlying attention: A resting state study. Behavioural Brain Research, 278, 315-322.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.10.002URLpmid: 25311282
46 Wang X., Zhao X., Gui X., & Chen A . ( 2016). Alertness function of thalamus in conflict adaptation. NeuroImage, 132, 274-282.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.048URLpmid: 26908318
47 Xuan B., Mackie M. A., Spagna A., Wu T., Tian Y., Hof P. R., & Fan J . ( 2016). The activation of interactive attentional networks. NeuroImage, 129, 308-319.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.017URLpmid: 26794640
48 Yamada Y., Kawabe T., & Miura K . ( 2012). One's own name distorts visual space. Neuroscience Letters, 531( 2), 96-98.
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.028URLpmid: 23127857
49 Zhu M., Hu Y., Tang X., Luo J., & Gao X . ( 2015). Withholding response to self-face is faster than to other- face. Journal of Motor Behavior, 47( 2), 117-123.
doi: 10.1080/00222895.2014.959888URLpmid: 25356599




[1]王慧媛, 陈艾睿, 张明. 意义关联的注意定向效应:基于空间位置的抑制和捕获[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(2): 113-127.
[2]罗禹,念靖晴,鲍未,张静静,赵守盈,潘运,许爽,张禹. 急性应激损害对威胁刺激的注意解除[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 26-37.
[3]任小云,李玉婷,毛伟宾,耿秋晨. 情绪对连续事件定向遗忘的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(3): 269-279.
[4]张燕, 曹慧敏, 郑元杰, 任衍具. 自上而下的目标调节奖赏联结干扰子 的注意定向和脱离[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(4): 377-389.
[5]黄敏学;王艺婷; 廖俊云;刘茂红. 评论不一致性对消费者的双面影响:产品属性与调节定向的调节[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(3): 370-382.
[6]杨文琪;李强;郭名扬;范谦;何伊丽. 权力感对个体的影响:调节定向的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(3): 404-415.
[7]耿晓伟, 姜宏艺. 调节定向和调节匹配对情感预测中 影响偏差的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(12): 1537-1547.
[8]周希;宛小昂;杜頔康;熊异雷;黄蔚欣. 不连续虚拟现实空间中的再定向[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(8): 924-932.
[9]王爱君;李毕琴;张明. 三维空间深度位置上基于空间的返回抑制[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(7): 859-868.
[10]李晔;张文慧;龙立荣. 自我牺牲型领导对下属工作绩效的影响机制 ——战略定向与领导认同的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(5): 653-662.
[11]杜晓梦, 赵占波, 崔晓. 评论效价、新产品类型与调节定向对在线评论有用性的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(4): 555-568.
[12]汪涛;谢志鹏;崔楠. 和品牌聊聊天 —— 拟人化沟通对消费者品牌态度影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(7): 987-999.
[13]王瑛瑛;梁九清;郭春彦. 单字法定向遗忘中情绪指示符对记忆编码过程的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(6): 740-753.
[14]杨文静;刘培朵;崔茜;郝鑫;肖宵;张庆林. 自我参照对情绪性记忆定向遗忘的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(2): 156-164.
[15]房慧聪,周琳. 性别、寻路策略与导航方式对寻路行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(8): 1058-1065.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4322
相关话题/网络 实验 空间 心理 中央