1 上海师范大学教育学院心理系, 上海200234
2 南京工业大学社会工作与管理系, 南京 211816
3 南京工业大学社会创新与发展研究所, 南京 211816
收稿日期:
2017-10-17出版日期:
2018-11-30发布日期:
2018-10-30Cognitive advantage for self-information: Evidence from the orienting network of attention
GAO Hong1, LI Yangzhuo1, HU Die1, ZHU Min2,3, GAO Xiangping1, HU Tianyi1()1 Department of Psychology, School of Education, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai 200234, China
2 Department of Social Work and Management, Nanjing Tech University, Nanjing 211816, China
3 Institute of Social Innovation and Development, Nanjing Tech University, Nanjing 211816, China
Received:
2017-10-17Online:
2018-11-30Published:
2018-10-30摘要/Abstract
摘要: 研究采用注意网络测验任务(attention network test, ANT), 分别检验自我相关信息在注意的警觉、定向和执行控制网络上的加工效率差异, 以揭示自我信息识别优势的注意机制。实验1考察了面孔类型(自我面孔或他人面孔)在3种注意网络下的加工效率差异; 实验2采用自我联结学习范式对实验1进行验证; 实验3将颜色类型(红色或绿色)作为目标刺激, 面孔类型作为背景, 以考察任务无关自我信息是否对注意网络加工效率有影响。研究结果发现, 当自我信息为目标时, 个体在注意定向网络上存在加工效率优势, 而警觉和执行控制网络上没有加工效率差异。当自我相关信息与任务无关时, 在注意的警觉、定向和执行控制网络中均未表现出加工效率优势。说明了自我信息的注意优势发生在注意定向网络上, 且受任务优先性影响。
图/表 9
图1注意网络测验单个试次示意图
图1注意网络测验单个试次示意图
表1实验1各条件下被试识别面孔的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 自我面孔 | 朋友面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 684 ± 59 | 97.32 ± 3.96 | 714 ± 65 | 97.32 ± 6.02 |
双重线索 | 636 ± 60 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | 677 ± 63 | 98.21 ± 4.16 | |
中央线索 | 640 ± 56 | 96.13 ± 5.77 | 683 ± 59 | 98.51 ± 3.25 | |
空间线索 | 587 ± 62 | 97.92 ± 4.32 | 633 ± 61 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | |
平均值 | 636 ± 53 | 97.25 ± 2.60 | 676 ± 54 | 97.92 ± 2.78 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 698 ± 58 | 91.13 ± 6.20 | 723 ± 45 | 97.02 ± 5.18 |
双重线索 | 654 ± 65 | 97.02 ± 4.66 | 686 ± 60 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | |
中央线索 | 670 ± 63 | 94.94 ± 7.64 | 698 ± 65 | 95.83 ± 6.99 | |
空间线索 | 596 ± 73 | 98.21 ± 4.12 | 653 ± 65 | 98.21 ± 4.16 | |
平均值 | 654 ± 56 | 96.80 ± 3.08 | 690 ± 51 | 97.17 ± 3.22 |
表1实验1各条件下被试识别面孔的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 自我面孔 | 朋友面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 684 ± 59 | 97.32 ± 3.96 | 714 ± 65 | 97.32 ± 6.02 |
双重线索 | 636 ± 60 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | 677 ± 63 | 98.21 ± 4.16 | |
中央线索 | 640 ± 56 | 96.13 ± 5.77 | 683 ± 59 | 98.51 ± 3.25 | |
空间线索 | 587 ± 62 | 97.92 ± 4.32 | 633 ± 61 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | |
平均值 | 636 ± 53 | 97.25 ± 2.60 | 676 ± 54 | 97.92 ± 2.78 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 698 ± 58 | 91.13 ± 6.20 | 723 ± 45 | 97.02 ± 5.18 |
双重线索 | 654 ± 65 | 97.02 ± 4.66 | 686 ± 60 | 97.62 ± 4.45 | |
中央线索 | 670 ± 63 | 94.94 ± 7.64 | 698 ± 65 | 95.83 ± 6.99 | |
空间线索 | 596 ± 73 | 98.21 ± 4.12 | 653 ± 65 | 98.21 ± 4.16 | |
平均值 | 654 ± 56 | 96.80 ± 3.08 | 690 ± 51 | 97.17 ± 3.22 |
图2实验1中自我和朋友面孔在注意网络的加工效率注: 两种面孔在注意网络的平均反应时, *p = 0.02误差线: 95%可信区间
图2实验1中自我和朋友面孔在注意网络的加工效率注: 两种面孔在注意网络的平均反应时, *p = 0.02误差线: 95%可信区间
图3图形-身份标签联结学习任务流程图(a)及ANT目标屏示例(b)
图3图形-身份标签联结学习任务流程图(a)及ANT目标屏示例(b)
表2加工不同身份图形的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 自我图形 | 朋友图形 | 无社会意义图形 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 613 ± 64 | 98.33 ± 3.39 | 627 ± 58 | 97.50 ± 4.97 | 647 ± 80 | 97.22 ± 5.03 |
双重线索 | 553 ± 64 | 98.06 ± 3.58 | 562 ± 62 | 97.22 ± 4.55 | 581 ± 76 | 96.39 ± 5.66 | |
中央线索 | 563 ± 62 | 96.39 ± 4.20 | 566 ± 67 | 96.67 ± 4.69 | 570 ± 76 | 97.78 ± 6.16 | |
空间线索 | 526 ± 62 | 96.67 ± 6.03 | 549 ± 62 | 97.50 ± 4.46 | 572 ± 81 | 96.95 ± 4.63 | |
平均值 | 564 ± 58 | 97.36 ± 3.32 | 576 ± 59 | 97.22 ± 3.30 | 593 ± 71 | 97.08 ± 4.53 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 635 ± 71 | 96.11 ± 6.09 | 634 ± 58 | 98.06 ± 4.20 | 649 ± 68 | 98.06 ± 3.58 |
双重线索 | 574 ± 62 | 98.33 ± 3.39 | 587 ± 68 | 96.94 ± 5.99 | 602 ± 68 | 98.61 ± 3.84 | |
中央线索 | 587 ± 54 | 98.06 ± 4.20 | 584 ± 56 | 97.22 ± 5.93 | 608 ± 72 | 97.78 ± 4.86 | |
空间线索 | 550 ± 59 | 97.78 ± 4.86 | 575 ± 61 | 98.61 ± 3.84 | 585 ± 68 | 97.22 ± 5.05 | |
平均值 | 586 ± 57 | 97.57 ± 3.33 | 595 ± 56 | 97.71 ± 3.60 | 611 ± 65 | 97.92 ± 3.37 |
表2加工不同身份图形的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 自我图形 | 朋友图形 | 无社会意义图形 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 613 ± 64 | 98.33 ± 3.39 | 627 ± 58 | 97.50 ± 4.97 | 647 ± 80 | 97.22 ± 5.03 |
双重线索 | 553 ± 64 | 98.06 ± 3.58 | 562 ± 62 | 97.22 ± 4.55 | 581 ± 76 | 96.39 ± 5.66 | |
中央线索 | 563 ± 62 | 96.39 ± 4.20 | 566 ± 67 | 96.67 ± 4.69 | 570 ± 76 | 97.78 ± 6.16 | |
空间线索 | 526 ± 62 | 96.67 ± 6.03 | 549 ± 62 | 97.50 ± 4.46 | 572 ± 81 | 96.95 ± 4.63 | |
平均值 | 564 ± 58 | 97.36 ± 3.32 | 576 ± 59 | 97.22 ± 3.30 | 593 ± 71 | 97.08 ± 4.53 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 635 ± 71 | 96.11 ± 6.09 | 634 ± 58 | 98.06 ± 4.20 | 649 ± 68 | 98.06 ± 3.58 |
双重线索 | 574 ± 62 | 98.33 ± 3.39 | 587 ± 68 | 96.94 ± 5.99 | 602 ± 68 | 98.61 ± 3.84 | |
中央线索 | 587 ± 54 | 98.06 ± 4.20 | 584 ± 56 | 97.22 ± 5.93 | 608 ± 72 | 97.78 ± 4.86 | |
空间线索 | 550 ± 59 | 97.78 ± 4.86 | 575 ± 61 | 98.61 ± 3.84 | 585 ± 68 | 97.22 ± 5.05 | |
平均值 | 586 ± 57 | 97.57 ± 3.33 | 595 ± 56 | 97.71 ± 3.60 | 611 ± 65 | 97.92 ± 3.37 |
图4不同意义图形的注意网络加工效率注:三种不同意义图形在注意网络的平均反应时, *p < 0.001误差线:95%可信区间
图4不同意义图形的注意网络加工效率注:三种不同意义图形在注意网络的平均反应时, *p < 0.001误差线:95%可信区间
图5实验3中目标及侧翼冲突示例注:彩图见电子版
图5实验3中目标及侧翼冲突示例注:彩图见电子版
表3实验3各条件下被试识别颜色的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 红色 | 绿色 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 664 ± 114 | 97.92 ± 3.68 | 674 ± 116 | 98.26 ± 5.48 |
双重线索 | 683 ± 104 | 98.61 ± 3.17 | 693 ± 116 | 98.61 ± 4.01 | |
中央线索 | 632 ± 117 | 96.88 ± 5.39 | 643 ± 123 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | |
空间线索 | 695 ± 114 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | 702 ± 110 | 98.61 ±3.17 | |
平均值 | 678 ± 111 | 97.66 ± 2.07 | 669 ± 105 | 98.35 ± 2.30 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 656 ± 119 | 96.53 ± 6.46 | 678 ± 117 | 97.92 ± 4.43 |
双重线索 | 614 ± 123 | 97.57 ± 4.58 | 628 ± 142 | 98.61 ± 3.17 | |
中央线索 | 640 ± 122 | 96.18 ± 5.48 | 637 ± 125 | 97.22 ± 5.31 | |
空间线索 | 630 ± 116 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | 650 ± 133 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | |
平均值 | 648 ± 124 | 97.31 ± 2.34 | 635 ± 116 | 97.74 ± 2.67 |
表3实验3各条件下被试识别颜色的反应时(ms)和正确率(%) (M ± SD)
侧翼类型 | 线索类型 | 红色 | 绿色 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
反应时 | 正确率 | 反应时 | 正确率 | ||
侧翼一致 | 无线索 | 664 ± 114 | 97.92 ± 3.68 | 674 ± 116 | 98.26 ± 5.48 |
双重线索 | 683 ± 104 | 98.61 ± 3.17 | 693 ± 116 | 98.61 ± 4.01 | |
中央线索 | 632 ± 117 | 96.88 ± 5.39 | 643 ± 123 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | |
空间线索 | 695 ± 114 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | 702 ± 110 | 98.61 ±3.17 | |
平均值 | 678 ± 111 | 97.66 ± 2.07 | 669 ± 105 | 98.35 ± 2.30 | |
侧翼冲突 | 无线索 | 656 ± 119 | 96.53 ± 6.46 | 678 ± 117 | 97.92 ± 4.43 |
双重线索 | 614 ± 123 | 97.57 ± 4.58 | 628 ± 142 | 98.61 ± 3.17 | |
中央线索 | 640 ± 122 | 96.18 ± 5.48 | 637 ± 125 | 97.22 ± 5.31 | |
空间线索 | 630 ± 116 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | 650 ± 133 | 98.96 ± 2.81 | |
平均值 | 648 ± 124 | 97.31 ± 2.34 | 635 ± 116 | 97.74 ± 2.67 |
图6自我和朋友面孔注意网络的加工效率注:误差线:95%可信区间
图6自我和朋友面孔注意网络的加工效率注:误差线:95%可信区间
参考文献 49
1 | Callejas A., Lupià?ez J., Funes M. J., & Tudela P . ( 2005). Modulations among the alerting, orienting and executive control networks. Experimental Brain Research, 167( 1), 27-37. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2365-zURLpmid: 16021429 |
2 | Corbetta M., &Shulman , G. L . ( 2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3( 3), 201-215. |
3 | Corbetta M., Patel G., & Shulman G. L . ( 2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58( 3), 306-324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017URLpmid: 18466742 |
4 | Crottaz-Herbette S., &Menon V. , ( 2006). Where and when the anterior cingulate cortex modulates attentional response: Combined fMRI and ERP evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18( 5), 766-780. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.5.766URLpmid: 16768376 |
5 | Devue C., &Brédart S. , ( 2008). Attention to self-referential stimuli: Can I ignore my own face? Acta Psychologica, 128( 2), 290-297. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.004URLpmid: 18413272 |
6 | Devue C., Laloyaux C., Feyers D., Theeuwes J., & Brédart S . ( 2009). Do pictures of faces, and which ones, capture attention in the inattentional-blindness paradigm? Perception, 38( 4), 552-568. |
7 | Devue C., Van der Stigchel S., Brédart S., & Theeuwes J . ( 2009). You do not find your own face faster; you just look at it longer. Cognition, 111( 1), 114-122. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.003URLpmid: 19230872 |
8 | Engle R.W., &Kane , M. J . ( 2003). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 145-199. |
9 | Fan J., McCandliss B. D., Fossella J., Flombaum J. I., & Posner M. I . ( 2005). The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26( 2), 471-479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.004URLpmid: 15907304 |
10 | Fan J., McCandliss B. D., Sommer T., Raz A., & Posner M. I . ( 2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14( 3), 340-347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886URLpmid: 11970796 |
11 | Farrant K., &Uddin , L. Q . ( 2015). Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention networks in the human brain. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 165-174. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001URLpmid: 4396619 |
12 | Federico F., Marotta A., Adriani T., Maccari L., & Casagrande M . ( 2013). Attention network test — The impact of social information on executive control, alerting and orienting. Acta Psychologica, 143( 1), 65-70. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.02.006URLpmid: 23542806 |
13 | Gao W., Gilmore J. H., Shen D., Smith J. K., Zhu H., & Lin W . ( 2013). The synchronization within and interaction between the default and dorsal attention networks in early infancy. Cerebral Cortex, 23( 3), 594-603. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs043URLpmid: 3563337 |
14 | Garza J. P., Strom M. J., Wright C. E., Roberts R. J., & Reed C. L . ( 2013). Top-down influences mediate hand bias in spatial attention. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75( 5), 819-823. doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0480-7URLpmid: 23722884 |
15 | Hackley S.A., &Valle-Inclán F. , ( 1999). Accessory stimulus effects on response selection: Does arousal speed decision making? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11( 3), 321-329. doi: 10.1162/089892999563427URLpmid: 10402259 |
16 | Hackley S.A., &Valle-Inclán F. , ( 2003). Which stages of processing are speeded by a warning signal? Biological Psychology, 64( 1-2), 27-45. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00101-7URLpmid: 14602354 |
17 | Haykin S., Fatemi M., Setoodeh P., & Xue Y . ( 2012). Cognitive control. Proceedings of the IEEE, 100( 12), 3156-3169. |
18 | Indovina I. &Macaluso E. , ( 2007). Dissociation of stimulus relevance and saliency factors during shifts of visuospatial attention. Cerebral Cortex, 17( 7), 1701-1711. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl081URLpmid: 17003078 |
19 | Jannati A., Gaspar J. M., & McDonald J. J . ( 2013). Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39( 6), 1713-1730. doi: 10.1037/a0032251URLpmid: 23527999 |
20 | Keyes H. &Dlugokencka A. , ( 2014). Do I have my attention? Speed of processing advantages for the self-face are not driven by automatic attention capture. PLoS One, 9( 10), e110792. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110792URLpmid: 4206440 |
21 | Kim J., Kang M. S., Cho Y. S., & Lee S. H . ( 2017). Prolonged interruption of cognitive control of conflict processing over human faces by task-irrelevant emotion expression. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1024. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01024URLpmid: 5476788 |
22 | Kratz O., Studer P., Malcherek S., Erbe K., Moll G. H., & Heinrich H . ( 2011). Attentional processes in children with ADHD: An event-related potential study using the attention network test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 81( 2), 82-90. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.05.008URLpmid: 21641942 |
23 | Li H., &Cai H.D, . ( 2013). The modulation of emotion on the attentional function networks. Advances in Psychological Science, 21( 1), 59-67. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00059URL |
24 | [ 李贺, 蔡厚德 . ( 2013). 情绪对注意功能网络的调制. 心理科学进展, 21( 1), 59-67.] doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00059URL |
25 | Liu M. H., Wang L. Y., Sui J., & Zhang M . ( 2012). Modulation of self-face for visual spatial attention: Evidence from a Posner’s cueing paradigm. Journal of Psychological Science, 35( 1), 24-29. |
26 | Mackie M. A., Dam N. T. V., & Fan J . ( 2013). Cognitive control and attentional functions. Brain and Cognition, 82( 3), 301-312. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.004URLpmid: 23792472 |
27 | Moray N.., ( 1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11( 1), 56-60. doi: 10.1080/17470215908416289URL |
28 | Mueller. C.J., &Kuchinke , L. ( 2016). Processing of face identity in the affective flanker task: A diffusion model analysis. Psychological Research, 80( 6), 1-11. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0696-3URLpmid: 26253324 |
29 | Neuhaus A. H., Urbanek C., Opgen-Rhein C., Hahn E., Ta T. M. T., Koehler S., .. Dettling M . ( 2010). Event-related potentials associated with attention network test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 76( 2), 72-79. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.02.005URLpmid: 20184924 |
30 | Ninomiya H., Onitsuka T., Chen C. H., Sato E., & Tashiro N . ( 1998). P300 in response to the subject’s own face. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 52( 5), 519-522. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.1998.00445.xURLpmid: 10215014 |
31 | Palermo R., & Rhodes G . ( 2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia, 45( 1), 75-92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.025URLpmid: 16797607 |
32 | Pannese A., &Hirsch J. , ( 2010). Self-specific priming effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 19( 4), 962-968. |
33 | Petersen, S. E., &Posner , M. I . ( 2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35( 5), 73-89. |
34 | Posner, M. I., &Petersen , S. E . ( 1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42. |
35 | Rothbart, M. K., &Posner , M. I . ( 2015). The developing brain in a multitasking world. Developmental Review, 35, 42-63. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.006URLpmid: 25821335 |
36 | Shulman G. L., D'Avossa G., Tansy A. P., & Corbetta M . ( 2002). Two attentional processes in the parietal lobe. Cerebral Cortex, 12( 11), 1124-1131. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.11.1124URLpmid: 12379601 |
37 | Spagna A., Martella D., Sebastiani M., Maccari L., Marotta A., & Casagrande M . ( 2014). Efficiency and interactions of alerting, orienting and executive networks: The impact of imperative stimulus type. Acta Psychologica, 148( 3), 209-215. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.007URLpmid: 24607440 |
38 | Sui J., Liu C. H., Wang L., & Han S . ( 2009). Attentional orientation induced by temporarily established self- referential cues. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62( 5), 844-849. doi: 10.1080/17470210802559393URLpmid: 19132633 |
39 | Sui J., Rotshtein P., & Humphreys G. W . ( 2013). Coupling social attention to the self forms a network for personal significance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110( 19), 7607-7612. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221862110URL |
40 | Sui J., Zhu Y., & Han S. H . ( 2006). Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: An event-related brain potential study. Neuroreport, 17( 4), 423-427. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000203357.65190.61URLpmid: 16514370 |
41 | Tang X., Wu J., & Shen Y . ( 2006). The interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and exogenous attention. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 61( 14), 208-224 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.002URLpmid: 26546734 |
42 | Tong F.., &Nakayama , K. ( 1999). Robust representations for faces: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25( 4), 1016-1035. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.25.4.1016URLpmid: 10464943 |
43 | Turk D. J., Brady-van Den Bos M., Collard P., Gillespie- Smith K., Conway M. A., & Cunningham S. J . ( 2013). Divided attention selectively impairs memory for self- relevant information. Memory & Cognition, 41( 4), 503-510. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0279-0URLpmid: 23263878 |
44 | Uddin L.Q . ( 2015). Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16( 1), 55-61. doi: 10.1038/nrn3857URLpmid: 25406711 |
45 | Visintin E., De Panfilis C., Antonucci C., Capecci C., Marchesi C., & Sambataro F . ( 2015). Parsing the intrinsic networks underlying attention: A resting state study. Behavioural Brain Research, 278, 315-322. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.10.002URLpmid: 25311282 |
46 | Wang X., Zhao X., Gui X., & Chen A . ( 2016). Alertness function of thalamus in conflict adaptation. NeuroImage, 132, 274-282. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.048URLpmid: 26908318 |
47 | Xuan B., Mackie M. A., Spagna A., Wu T., Tian Y., Hof P. R., & Fan J . ( 2016). The activation of interactive attentional networks. NeuroImage, 129, 308-319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.017URLpmid: 26794640 |
48 | Yamada Y., Kawabe T., & Miura K . ( 2012). One's own name distorts visual space. Neuroscience Letters, 531( 2), 96-98. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.028URLpmid: 23127857 |
49 | Zhu M., Hu Y., Tang X., Luo J., & Gao X . ( 2015). Withholding response to self-face is faster than to other- face. Journal of Motor Behavior, 47( 2), 117-123. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2014.959888URLpmid: 25356599 |
相关文章 15
[1] | 王慧媛, 陈艾睿, 张明. 意义关联的注意定向效应:基于空间位置的抑制和捕获[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(2): 113-127. |
[2] | 罗禹,念靖晴,鲍未,张静静,赵守盈,潘运,许爽,张禹. 急性应激损害对威胁刺激的注意解除[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 26-37. |
[3] | 任小云,李玉婷,毛伟宾,耿秋晨. 情绪对连续事件定向遗忘的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(3): 269-279. |
[4] | 张燕, 曹慧敏, 郑元杰, 任衍具. 自上而下的目标调节奖赏联结干扰子 的注意定向和脱离[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(4): 377-389. |
[5] | 黄敏学;王艺婷; 廖俊云;刘茂红. 评论不一致性对消费者的双面影响:产品属性与调节定向的调节[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(3): 370-382. |
[6] | 杨文琪;李强;郭名扬;范谦;何伊丽. 权力感对个体的影响:调节定向的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(3): 404-415. |
[7] | 耿晓伟, 姜宏艺. 调节定向和调节匹配对情感预测中 影响偏差的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(12): 1537-1547. |
[8] | 周希;宛小昂;杜頔康;熊异雷;黄蔚欣. 不连续虚拟现实空间中的再定向[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(8): 924-932. |
[9] | 王爱君;李毕琴;张明. 三维空间深度位置上基于空间的返回抑制[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(7): 859-868. |
[10] | 李晔;张文慧;龙立荣. 自我牺牲型领导对下属工作绩效的影响机制 ——战略定向与领导认同的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(5): 653-662. |
[11] | 杜晓梦, 赵占波, 崔晓. 评论效价、新产品类型与调节定向对在线评论有用性的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(4): 555-568. |
[12] | 汪涛;谢志鹏;崔楠. 和品牌聊聊天 —— 拟人化沟通对消费者品牌态度影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(7): 987-999. |
[13] | 王瑛瑛;梁九清;郭春彦. 单字法定向遗忘中情绪指示符对记忆编码过程的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(6): 740-753. |
[14] | 杨文静;刘培朵;崔茜;郝鑫;肖宵;张庆林. 自我参照对情绪性记忆定向遗忘的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(2): 156-164. |
[15] | 房慧聪,周琳. 性别、寻路策略与导航方式对寻路行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(8): 1058-1065. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4322