北京大学心理与认知科学学院和行为与心理健康北京市重点实验室, 北京 100871
收稿日期:
2017-11-22出版日期:
2018-09-15发布日期:
2018-07-27基金资助:
国家自然科学基金项目(31571134)Pay-forward effect of resource allocation in preschoolers: Role of theory of mind and empathy
XIE Dongjie, LU Hao, SU Yanjie()School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China
Received:
2017-11-22Online:
2018-09-15Published:
2018-07-27摘要/Abstract
摘要: 当他人对个体进行资源上的自私、公平或慷慨分配后, 个体倾向于以同样的分配方式对待第三个无关个体, 这一现象为资源分配模式的传递效应。研究考察自我绝对利益受到影响时这一效应在学龄前儿童中的表现以及其中可能的社会认知机制。118名4~6岁儿童被随机分到实验条件或对照条件中。实验条件下被试与匿名同伴合作完成拼图游戏后得到同伴分配的1个(自私)、3个(公平)或5个(慷慨)代币(共6个), 之后再作为分配者与另一匿名接受者完成同样的游戏; 而对照条件下被试只扮演分配者完成拼图和分配游戏。结果发现, 公平条件下公平分配比例与对照条件之间不存在显著差异, 而自私条件下自私分配模式比例和慷慨条件下慷慨分配模式比例分别高于对照条件下相应比例; 在慷慨条件下传递者比非传递者拥有更强的二级心理理论与(认知)共情能力。这提示学龄前儿童会传递自私和慷慨分配模式, 心理理论和共情更强的学龄前儿童更有可能传递慷慨分配模式。
图/表 4
表1不同条件下学龄前儿童分配给他人的代币数量 [M (SD)]
条件 | 男生 | 女生 | 总计 |
---|---|---|---|
对照条件 | 2.87 (0.52) | 2.87 (0.35) | 2.87 (0.43) |
自私条件 | 2.00 (0.78) | 2.57 (0.65) | 2.29 (0.76) |
公平条件 | 2.77 (0.60) | 2.82 (0.64) | 2.80 (0.61) |
慷慨条件 | 3.20 (0.78) | 3.53 (1.46) | 3.37 (1.16) |
表1不同条件下学龄前儿童分配给他人的代币数量 [M (SD)]
条件 | 男生 | 女生 | 总计 |
---|---|---|---|
对照条件 | 2.87 (0.52) | 2.87 (0.35) | 2.87 (0.43) |
自私条件 | 2.00 (0.78) | 2.57 (0.65) | 2.29 (0.76) |
公平条件 | 2.77 (0.60) | 2.82 (0.64) | 2.80 (0.61) |
慷慨条件 | 3.20 (0.78) | 3.53 (1.46) | 3.37 (1.16) |
图1各实验条件与对照条件中不同分配模式的人数所占比例
图1各实验条件与对照条件中不同分配模式的人数所占比例
表2心理理论、共情与分配数量的皮尔逊相关系数
各组分配数量 | 心理理论 | 共情 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
一级 | 二级 | 总分 | 情感 | 认知 | 行为 | 平均分 | |
对照条件 | 0.13 | 0.12 | 0.17 | -0.03 | 0.26 | 0.19 | 0.14 |
自私条件 | 0.08 | 0.24 | 0.24 | 0.09 | -0.24 | -0.07 | -0.06 |
公平条件 | -0.04 | -0.01 | -0.03 | -0.23 | -0.10 | -0.11 | -0.18 |
慷慨条件 | -0.06 | 0.31? | 0.18 | 0.27 | 0.32? | 0.29 | 0.41* |
所有被试 | -0.02 | 0.18* | 0.11 | 0.03 | -0.05 | 0.05 | 0.00 |
表2心理理论、共情与分配数量的皮尔逊相关系数
各组分配数量 | 心理理论 | 共情 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
一级 | 二级 | 总分 | 情感 | 认知 | 行为 | 平均分 | |
对照条件 | 0.13 | 0.12 | 0.17 | -0.03 | 0.26 | 0.19 | 0.14 |
自私条件 | 0.08 | 0.24 | 0.24 | 0.09 | -0.24 | -0.07 | -0.06 |
公平条件 | -0.04 | -0.01 | -0.03 | -0.23 | -0.10 | -0.11 | -0.18 |
慷慨条件 | -0.06 | 0.31? | 0.18 | 0.27 | 0.32? | 0.29 | 0.41* |
所有被试 | -0.02 | 0.18* | 0.11 | 0.03 | -0.05 | 0.05 | 0.00 |
表3不同条件下学龄前儿童的心理理论与共情得分[M (SD)]
实验条件 | 心理理论 | 共情 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
一级 | 二级 | 总分 | 情感 | 认知 | 行为 | 平均分 | |
自私条件(n = 28) | 1.75 | 1.18 | 2.93 | 0.64 | 1.59 | 1.80 | 1.20 |
(0.65) | (0.72) | (0.94) | (1.32) | (1.38) | (1.14) | (1.13) | |
非传递者(n = 13) | 1.77 | 1.38 | 3.15 | 0.65 | 1.37 | 1.77 | 1.13 |
(0.60) | (0.65) | (0.80) | (1.15) | (1.53) | (1.24) | (1.16) | |
传递者(n = 15) | 1.73 | 1.00 | 2.73 | 0.63 | 1.77 | 1.83 | 1.27 |
(0.70) | (0.76) | (1.03) | (1.49) | (1.26) | (1.08) | (1.15) | |
公平条件(n = 30) | 1.57 | 0.97 | 2.53 | 1.05 | 1.45 | 2.11 | 1.42 |
(0.77) | (0.85) | (1.48) | (1.47) | (1.42) | (1.17) | (1.24) | |
非传递者(n = 6) | 1.33 | 0.67 | 2.00 | 1.17 | 1.53 | 2.21 | 1.52 |
(0.82) | (1.03) | (1.67) | (1.75) | (1.61) | (1.45) | (1.56) | |
传递者(n = 24) | 1.63 | 1.04 | 2.67 | 1.02 | 1.43 | 2.08 | 1.39 |
(0.77) | (0.81) | (1.44) | (1.43) | (1.41) | (1.13) | (1.19) | |
慷慨条件(n = 30) | 1.67 | 1.27 | 2.93 | 0.49 | 0.70 | 1.78 | 0.85 |
(0.71) | (0.79) | (1.11) | (1.08) | (1.10) | (0.73) | (0.71) | |
非传递者(n = 20) | 1.70 | 1.05 | 2.75 | 0.24 | 0.39 | 1.67 | 0.62 |
(0.66) | (0.83) | (1.21) | (1.15) | (0.89) | (0.80) | (0.61) | |
传递者(n = 10) | 1.60 | 1.70 | 3.30 | 0.99 | 1.32 | 2.00 | 1.32 |
(0.84) | (0.48) | (0.82) | (0.72) | (1.26) | (0.53) | (0.69) | |
对照条件(n = 30) | 1.73 | 1.27 | 3.00 | 0.88 | 1.25 | 2.21 | 1.30 |
(0.58) | (0.69) | (0.95) | (1.29) | (1.44) | (1.10) | (1.01) | |
所有被试(N = 118) | 1.68 | 1.17 | 2.85 | 0.77 | 1.24 | 1.98 | 1.19 |
(0.68) | (0.77) | (1.14) | (1.30) | (1.37) | (1.05) | (1.05) |
表3不同条件下学龄前儿童的心理理论与共情得分[M (SD)]
实验条件 | 心理理论 | 共情 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
一级 | 二级 | 总分 | 情感 | 认知 | 行为 | 平均分 | |
自私条件(n = 28) | 1.75 | 1.18 | 2.93 | 0.64 | 1.59 | 1.80 | 1.20 |
(0.65) | (0.72) | (0.94) | (1.32) | (1.38) | (1.14) | (1.13) | |
非传递者(n = 13) | 1.77 | 1.38 | 3.15 | 0.65 | 1.37 | 1.77 | 1.13 |
(0.60) | (0.65) | (0.80) | (1.15) | (1.53) | (1.24) | (1.16) | |
传递者(n = 15) | 1.73 | 1.00 | 2.73 | 0.63 | 1.77 | 1.83 | 1.27 |
(0.70) | (0.76) | (1.03) | (1.49) | (1.26) | (1.08) | (1.15) | |
公平条件(n = 30) | 1.57 | 0.97 | 2.53 | 1.05 | 1.45 | 2.11 | 1.42 |
(0.77) | (0.85) | (1.48) | (1.47) | (1.42) | (1.17) | (1.24) | |
非传递者(n = 6) | 1.33 | 0.67 | 2.00 | 1.17 | 1.53 | 2.21 | 1.52 |
(0.82) | (1.03) | (1.67) | (1.75) | (1.61) | (1.45) | (1.56) | |
传递者(n = 24) | 1.63 | 1.04 | 2.67 | 1.02 | 1.43 | 2.08 | 1.39 |
(0.77) | (0.81) | (1.44) | (1.43) | (1.41) | (1.13) | (1.19) | |
慷慨条件(n = 30) | 1.67 | 1.27 | 2.93 | 0.49 | 0.70 | 1.78 | 0.85 |
(0.71) | (0.79) | (1.11) | (1.08) | (1.10) | (0.73) | (0.71) | |
非传递者(n = 20) | 1.70 | 1.05 | 2.75 | 0.24 | 0.39 | 1.67 | 0.62 |
(0.66) | (0.83) | (1.21) | (1.15) | (0.89) | (0.80) | (0.61) | |
传递者(n = 10) | 1.60 | 1.70 | 3.30 | 0.99 | 1.32 | 2.00 | 1.32 |
(0.84) | (0.48) | (0.82) | (0.72) | (1.26) | (0.53) | (0.69) | |
对照条件(n = 30) | 1.73 | 1.27 | 3.00 | 0.88 | 1.25 | 2.21 | 1.30 |
(0.58) | (0.69) | (0.95) | (1.29) | (1.44) | (1.10) | (1.01) | |
所有被试(N = 118) | 1.68 | 1.17 | 2.85 | 0.77 | 1.24 | 1.98 | 1.19 |
(0.68) | (0.77) | (1.14) | (1.30) | (1.37) | (1.05) | (1.05) |
参考文献 52
1 | Aguilar-Pardo D., Martínez-Arias R., & Colmenares F . ( 2013). The role of inhibition in young children’s altruistic behaviour. Cognitive Processing, 14( 3), 301-307. doi: 10.1007/s10339-013-0552-6URLpmid: 23436211 |
2 | Baumard N., Mascaro O., & Chevallier C . ( 2012). Preschoolers are able to take merit into account when distributing goods. Developmental Psychology, 48( 2), 492-498. doi: 10.1037/a0026598URLpmid: 22148948 |
3 | Beugré, C. D . ( 2009). Exploring the neural basis of fairness: A model of neuro-organizational justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110( 2), 129-139. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.06.005URL |
4 | Blake, P. R., & Rand, D. G . ( 2010). Currency value moderates equity preference among young children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31( 3), 210-218. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.06.012URL |
5 | Blake P. R., McAuliffe K., & Warneken F . ( 2014). The developmental origins of fairness: The knowledge-behavior gap. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18( 11), 559-561. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.08.003URLpmid: 25175834 |
6 | Cohen, J. ( Ed). (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. |
7 | Cowell J. M., Samek A., List J., & Decety J . ( 2015). The curious relation between theory of mind and sharing in preschool age children. PloS One, 10( 2), e0117947. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117947URLpmid: 25658696 |
8 | Dadds M. R., Hunter K., Hawes D. J., Frost A. D. J., Vassallo S., Bunn P., .. Masry Y. El . ( 2008). A measure of cognitive and affective empathy in children using parent ratings. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 39( 2), 111-122. doi: 10.1007/s10578-007-0075-4URLpmid: 17710538 |
9 | Davis, M. H . ( 2006). Empathy. In[J]. E. Stets & J. H. Turner (Eds.), Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions (pp. 443-466). Boston: Springer. |
10 | Decety, J., &Lamm, C. ( 2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. The Scientific World Journal, 6( 3), 1146-1163. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2006.221URLpmid: 16998603 |
11 | DeSteno D., Bartlett M. Y., Baumann J., Williams L. A., & Dickens L . ( 2010). Gratitude as moral sentiment: Emotion-guided cooperation in economic exchange. Emotion, 10( 2), 289-293. doi: 10.1037/a0017883URLpmid: 20364907 |
12 | Fehr, E., &Fischbacher, U. ( 2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425, 785-791. doi: 10.1038/nature02043URLpmid: 14574401 |
13 | Goldman, A. I . ( 1989). Interpretation psychologized. Mind & Language, 4( 3), 161-185. |
14 | Gray K., Ward A. F., & Norton M. I . ( 2014). Paying it forward: Generalized reciprocity and the limits of generosity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143( 1), 247-254. doi: 10.1037/a0031047URLpmid: 23244034 |
15 | Grocke P., Rossano F., & Tomasello M . ( 2015). Procedural justice in children: Preschoolers accept unequal resource distributions if the procedure provides equal opportunities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 197-210. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.008URL |
16 | Hamilton, I. M., & Taborsky, M. ( 2005). Contingent movement and cooperation evolve under generalized reciprocity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 272( 1578), 2259-2267. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3248URLpmid: 1560194 |
17 | Horita Y., Takezawa M., Kinjo T., Nakawake Y., & Masuda N . ( 2016). Transient nature of cooperation by pay-it-forward reciprocity. Scientific Reports, 6, 19471. doi: 10.1038/srep19471URLpmid: 4726336 |
18 | Kallgren C. A., Reno R. R., & Cialdini R. B . ( 2000). A focus theory of normative conduct: When norms do and do not affect behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26( 8), 1002-1012. doi: 10.1177/01461672002610009URL |
19 | Lamm C., Meltzoff A. N., & Decety J . ( 2010). How do we empathize with someone who is not like us? A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22( 2), 362-376. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21186URL |
20 | Leimgruber K. L., Ward A. F., Widness J., Norton M. I., Olson K. R., Gray K., & Santos L. R . ( 2014). Give what you get: Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and 4-year-old children pay forward positive and negative outcomes to conspecifics. PloS One, 9( 1), e87035. |
21 | McAuliffe K., Jordan J. J., & Warneken F . ( 2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 1-10. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.013URL |
22 | McAuliffe K., Raihani N. J., & Dunham Y . ( 2017). Children are sensitive to norms of giving. Cognition, 167, 151-159. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.006URLpmid: 28129890 |
23 | McCrink K., Bloom P., & Santos L. R . ( 2010). Children’s and adults’ judgments of equitable resource distributions. Developmental Science, 13( 1), 37-45. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00859.xURLpmid: 20121861 |
24 | Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. ( 2005). Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437, 1291-1298. doi: 10.1038/nature04131URL |
25 | Paulus, M. & Moore, C. ( 2015). Preschool children's anticipation of recipients’ emotions affects their resource allocation. Social Development, 24( 4), 852-867. doi: 10.1111/sode.12126URL |
26 | Premack, D., &Woodruff, G. ( 1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1( 4), 515-526. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512URL |
27 | Rutte, C., &Taborsky, M. ( 2007). Generalized reciprocity in rats. PLoS Biology, 5( 7), e196. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050196URL |
28 | Salali G. D., Juda M., & Henrich J . ( 2015). Transmission and development of costly punishment in children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36( 2), 86-94. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.09.004URL |
29 | Schug J., Takagishi H., Benech C., & Okada H . ( 2016). The development of theory of mind and positive and negative reciprocity in preschool children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 888. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00888URLpmid: 27445881 |
30 | Stanca, L. ( 2009). Measuring indirect reciprocity: Whose back do we scratch? Journal of Economic Psychology, 30( 2), 190-202. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2008.07.010URL |
31 | Steinbeis N., Bernhardt B. C., & Singer T . ( 2012). Impulse control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC mediate age-related and age-independent individual differences in strategic social behavior. Neuron, 73( 5), 1040-1051. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.027URL |
32 | Sullivan K., Zaitchik D., & Tager-Flusberg H . ( 1994). Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 30( 3), 395-402. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.3.395URL |
33 | Takagishi H., Kameshima S., Schug J., Koizumi M., & Yamagishi T . ( 2010). Theory of mind enhances preference for fairness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105( 1-2), 130-137. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.09.005URL |
34 | Wang, S., &Su, Y. J . ( 2013). From understanding to utilizing: Theory of mind and children’s distributive justice in different contexts. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45( 11), 1242-1250. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.01242URL |
35 | [ 王斯, 苏彦捷 . ( 2013). 从理解到使用:心理理论与儿童不同情境中的分配公平性. 心理学报, 45( 11), 1242-1250.] doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.01242URL |
36 | Wang Y. W., Zhang Z., Bai L. Y., Lin C. D., Osinsky R., & Hewig J . ( 2017). Ingroup/outgroup membership modulates fairness consideration: Neural signatures from ERPs and EEG oscillations. Scientific Reports, 7, 39827. doi: 10.1038/srep39827 |
37 | Wansink B., Kent R. J., & Hoch S. J . ( 1998). An anchoring and adjustment model of purchase quantity decisions. Journal of Marketing Research, 35( 1), 71-81. doi: 10.2307/3151931URL |
38 | Warneken F., Lohse K., Melis A. P., & Tomasello M . ( 2011). Young children share the spoils after collaboration. Psychological Science, 22( 2), 267-273. doi: 10.1177/0956797610395392URLpmid: 21196533 |
39 | Warneken, F., &Tomasello, M. ( 2013). The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116( 2), 338-350. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.002URLpmid: 23917162 |
40 | Watanabe T., Takezawa M., Nakawake Y., Kunimatsu A., Yamasue H., Nakamura M., .. Masuda N . ( 2014). Two distinct neural mechanisms underlying indirect reciprocity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111( 11), 3990-3995. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1318570111URLpmid: 24591599 |
41 | Wei, Q., &Su, Y. J . ( 2018). The development of different components of empathy during preschool years. Submitted. |
42 | [ 魏祺, 苏彦捷 . ( 2018). 共情的不同成分在学龄前儿童中的发展. 已投稿] |
43 | Wellman H. M., Cross D., & Watson J . ( 2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72( 3), 655-684. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00304URLpmid: 11405571 |
44 | Willis M. L., Lawson D. L., Ridley N. J., Koval P., & Rendell P. G . ( 2015). The contribution of emotional empathy to approachability judgments assigned to emotional faces is context specific. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1209. |
45 | Wu, Z., &Su, Y. J . ( 2014). How do preschoolers’ sharing behaviors relate to their theory of mind understanding? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 120, 73-86. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.007URLpmid: 24384326 |
46 | Wu Z., Zhang Z., Guo R., & Gros-Louis J . ( 2017). Motivation counts: Autonomous but not obligated sharing promotes happiness in preschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 867. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00867URLpmid: 28620328 |
47 | Yu, J., &Zhu, L. Q . ( 2010). The development of children’s fair behavior: Evidence from experimental games. Advances in Psychological Science, 18( 7), 1182-1188. |
48 | [ 于静, 朱莉琪 . ( 2010). 儿童公平行为的发展——来自博弈实验的证据. 心理科学进展, 18( 7), 1182-1188.] |
49 | Yu J., Zhu L. Q., & Leslie A. M . ( 2016). Children’s sharing behavior in Mini-Dictator games: The role of In-Group favoritism and theory of mind. Child Development, 87( 6), 1747-1757. doi: 10.1111/cdev.2016.87.issue-6URL |
50 | Yu, J. X., & Kou, Y. ( 2015). Paying it forward: The effect of selfishness transmission. Advances in Psychological Science, 23( 6), 1061-1069. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.01061URL |
51 | [ 余俊宣, 寇彧 . ( 2015). 自私行为的传递效应. 心理科学进展, 23( 6), 1061-1069.] |
52 | Zhang Q., Wang Y., Lui S. S. Y., Cheung E. F. C., Neumann D. L., Shum D. H. K., & Chan, R. C. K. ( 2014). Validation of the Griffith Empathy Measure in the Chinese context. Brain Impairment, 15( 1), 10-17. doi: 10.1017/BrImp.2014.1URL |
相关文章 15
[1] | 陈杰, 伍可, 史宇鹏, 艾小青. 特质性自我构念与内外群体疼痛共情的关系: 来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(6): 629-638. |
[2] | 李雄,李祚山,向滨洋,孟景. 注意线索对自闭特质个体疼痛共情的影响:来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(3): 294-306. |
[3] | 王文超,伍新春. 共情对灾后青少年亲社会行为的影响:感恩、社会支持和创伤后成长的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(3): 307-316. |
[4] | 黄亮,杨雪,黄志华,王益文. 理解双人肢体运动表达的友好和敌对意图的ERP证据[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(5): 557-570. |
[5] | 程家萍;罗跃嘉;崔芳. 认知负荷对疼痛共情的影响:来自ERP研究的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(5): 622-630. |
[6] | 宋娟; 郭丰波; 张振; 原胜; 金花; 王益文. 人际距离影响疼痛共情:朋友启动效应[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(7): 833-844. |
[7] | 黄四林;韩明跃;张梅;. 人际关系对社会责任感的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(5): 578-587. |
[8] | 何宁;朱云莉. 自爱与他爱:自恋、共情与内隐利他的关系[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(2): 199-210. |
[9] | 王雨晴;游旭群;焦健;谌鹏飞. 观点采择:基于自我的推理及其个体差异[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(8): 1039-1049. |
[10] | 黎翠红;何旭;郭春彦. 多特征刺激在视觉工作记忆中的存储模式[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(6): 734-745. |
[11] | 高雪梅;翁蕾;周群;赵偲;李芳. 暴力犯的疼痛共情更低:来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(4): 478-487. |
[12] | 钟罗金;范梦; 陈琳;王靖;莫雷;昂晨;林俊贤;庞慧然. 资源的交换价值和工资性质对其分配公平感的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(9): 1392-1399. |
[13] | 邓琳;罗品超;王俊芳;方俊聪;郑晓纯;李悦;郑希付. 不对称情绪分享过程中情绪反应自我指向增强效应的初探[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(8): 1103-1111. |
[14] | 马伟娜;朱蓓蓓. 孤独症儿童的情绪共情能力及情绪表情注意方式[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(4): 528-539. |
[15] | 冷英;卢家楣;金丽;潘发达;陈燕;沈永江. RSVP任务下双刺激重复的优势与劣势[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(3): 298-311. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4258