删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

跨期决策中的负折扣现象及其机制

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

孙红月(), 鲁盼, 蒋元萍
上海师范大学教育学院, 上海 200234
收稿日期:2020-07-20出版日期:2021-07-15发布日期:2021-05-24
通讯作者:孙红月E-mail:sunhy1215@126.com

基金资助:国家自然科学基金青年科学(71601121)

Negative discounting in intertemporal choice

SUN Hongyue(), LU Pan, JIANG Yuanping
College of Education, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai 200234, China
Received:2020-07-20Online:2021-07-15Published:2021-05-24
Contact:SUN Hongyue E-mail:sunhy1215@126.com






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 主流跨期决策模型认为, 跨期决策存在一个时间折扣过程。时间折扣是指人们会根据将来获益或损失的延迟时间对其效用进行折扣, 折扣后的效用小于原来的效用, 而负折扣现象违背了时间折扣过程。负折扣现象出现在金钱、非金钱的获益与损失领域, 其可能的解释机制为预期情绪。目前关于负折扣现象的研究, 较少验证其影响机制, 未来的研究可以结合眼动追踪和fMRI技术探讨其机制, 并丰富对负折扣现象的影响因素研究。


[1] 刘雪峰, 张志学, 梁钧平. (2007). 认知闭合需要、框架效应与决策偏好. 心理学报, 39(4), 611-618.
[2] 孙红月. (2014). 负折扣现象产生的心理机制研究 (博士学位论文). 中国科学院大学, 北京.
[3] 孙红月, 崔丽莹, 李丹. (2016). 负折扣现象: 跨期决策中的自我-他人决策差异. 心理科学, 39(4), 970-976.
[4] 孙红月, 江程铭. (2016). 跨期决策是基于选项还是基于维度? 心理科学进展, 24(3), 431-437.
[5] Abdellaoui, M., Gutierrez, C., & Kemel, E. (2018). Temporal discounting of gains and losses of time: An experimental investigation. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 57(1), 1-28.
doi: 10.1007/s11166-018-9287-1URL
[6] Amasino, D. R., Sullivan, N. J., Kranton, R. E., & Huettel, S. A. (2019). Amount and time exert independent influences on intertemporal choice. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(4), 383-392.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0537-2pmid: 30971787
[7] Antonides, G., & Wunderink, S. R. (2001). Subjective time preference and willingness to pay for an energy-saving durable good. Zeitschrift Für Sozialpsychologie, 32(3), 133-141.
doi: 10.1024//0044-3514.32.3.133URL
[8] Areni, C. S., & Black, I. (2015). Consumers’ responses to small portions: Signaling increases savoring and satiation. Psychology & Marketing, 32(5), 532-543.
doi: 10.1002/mar.20798URL
[9] Attaran, S., Attaran, M., & Kirkland, D. (2019, July). Savoring the product: A look at the impact of AI on pre and post purchase satisfaction. Paper presented at the meeting of Global Fashion Management Conference, Paris, France.
[10] Baker, F., Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2003). Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: Similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(3), 382-392.
doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.382URL
[11] Ballard, I. C., Kim, B., Liatsis, A., Aydogan, G., Cohen, J. D., & McClure, S. M. (2017). More is meaningful: The magnitude effect in intertemporal choice depends on self-control. Psychological Science, 28(10), 1443-1454.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617711455pmid: 28858559
[12] Barile, L., Cullis, J., & Jones, P. (2018). Time preference for investment in the Environment: The impact of intrinsic motivation. Economic Issues, 23(2), 31-56.
[13] Benhabib, J., Bisin, A., & Schotter, A. (2010). Present-bias, quasi-hyperbolic discounting, and fixed costs. Games & Economic Behavior, 69(2), 205-223.
[14] Berns, G. S., Chappelow, J., Cekic, M., Zink, C. F., Pagnoni, G., & Martin-Skurski, M. E. (2006). Neurobiological substrates of dread. Science, 312(5774), 754-758.
doi: 10.1126/science.1123721URL
[15] Black, I. R., & Areni, C. S. (2016). Anticipatory savoring and consumption: Just thinking about that first bite of chocolate fills you up faster. Psychology & Marketing, 33(7), 516-524.
doi: 10.1002/mar.2016.33.issue-7URL
[16] Chapman, G. B. (1996). Temporal discounting and utility for health and money. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(3), 771-791.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.3.771URL
[17] Chapman, G. B., & Coups, E. J. (1999). Time preferences and preventive health behavior: Acceptance of the influenza vaccine. Medical Decision Making, 19(3), 307-314.
pmid: 10424837
[18] di Cagno, D. T., Francesco, F., & Ashtiani, A. Z. (2019). Anticipatory feelings in intertemporal choice on consumption: A dynamic experiment. Psychology Research and Applications, 1(4), 89-103.
[19] Dixon, M. J., Victorino, L., Kwortnik, R. J., & Verma, R. (2017). Surprise, anticipation, and sequence effects in the design of experiential services. Production and Operations Management, 26(5), 945-960.
doi: 10.1111/poms.2017.26.issue-5URL
[20] Estle, S. J., Leonard, G., Joel, M., & Holt, D. D. (2006). Differential effects of amount on temporal and probability discounting of gains and losses. Memory & Cognition, 34(4), 914-928.
doi: 10.3758/BF03193437URL
[21] Faralla, V., Benuzzi, F., Lui, F., Baraldi, P., Dimitri, N., & Nichelli, P. (2015). Neural correlates in intertemporal choice of gains and losses. Journal of Neuroscience Psychology and Economics, 8(1), 27-47.
doi: 10.1037/npe0000032URL
[22] Franco-Watkins, A. M., Mattson, R. E., & Jackson, M. D. (2016). Now or later? Attentional processing and intertemporal choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(2- 3),206-217.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v29.1URL
[23] Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O'Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351-401.
doi: 10.1257/jel.40.2.351URL
[24] Fröhner, J. H., Teckentrup, V., Smolka, M. N., & Kroemer, N. B. (2019). Addressing the reliability fallacy in fMRI: Similar group effects may arise from unreliable individual effects. NeuroImage, 195, 174-189.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.053URL
[25] Ganguly, A., & Tasoff, J. (2017). Fantasy and dread: The demand for information and the consumption utility of the future. Management Science, 63(12), 4037-4060.
doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2016.2550URL
[26] Gard, D. E., Gard, M. G., Kring, A. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: A scale development study. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(6), 1086-1102.
doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.11.001URL
[27] Gluth, S., Hotaling, J. M., & Rieskamp, J. (2017). The attraction effect modulates reward prediction errors and intertemporal choices. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(2), 371-382.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2532-16.2016URL
[28] Hardisty, D. J., Appelt, K. C., & Weber, E. U. (2013). Good or bad, we want it now: Fixed-cost present bias for gains and losses explains magnitude asymmetries in intertemporal choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26(4), 348-361.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v26.4URL
[29] Hardisty, D. J., Frederick, S., & Weber, E. (2010, November). “I can’t stand waiting!” Dread looms larger than pleasurable anticipation. Paper presented at the meeting of The 31st Annual Conference of the Society for Judgment and Decision Making, St. Louis, MO.
[30] Hardisty, D. J., & Weber, E. U. (2009). Discounting future green: Money versus the environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 329-340.
doi: 10.1037/a0016433URL
[31] Hardisty, D. J., & Weber, E. U. (2020). Impatience and savoring versus dread: Asymmetries in anticipation explain consumer time preferences for positive versus negative events. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 30(4), 598-613.
doi: 10.1002/jcpy.v30.4URL
[32] Harris, C. R. (2011). Feelings of dread and intertemporal choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(1), 13-28.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.709URL
[33] Hirata, K., Ikeda, S., Ikefuji, M., Kang, M. I., & Yamada, K. (2017). Time discounting, ambiguity aversion, and preferences for future environmental policies: Evidence from discrete choice experiments. Retrieved October 23, 2017, from http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3046935.
[34] Ikeda, S. (2016). Varying impatience. In R. Sato (Ed.),The economics of self-destructive choices: Vol. 10: Advances in Japanese business and economics(pp.25-42). Tokyo, Japan: Springer-Verlag.
[35] Ikink, I., Engelmann, J. B., van den Bos, W., Roelofs, K., & Figner, B. (2019). Time ambiguity during intertemporal decision-making is aversive, impacting choice and neural value coding. NeuroImage, 185, 236-244.
doi: S1053-8119(18)31967-0pmid: 30296559
[36] King, J. A., Geisler, D., Bernardoni, F., Ritschel, F., Böhm, I., Seidel, M., ... Ehrlich, S. (2016). Altered neural efficiency of decision making during temporal reward discounting in anorexia nervosa. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(11), 972-979.
doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.08.005URL
[37] Laibson, D. (1997). Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 443-478.
doi: 10.1162/003355397555253URL
[38] Lempert, K. M., Glimcher, P. W., & Phelps, E. A. (2015). Emotional arousal and discount rate in intertemporal choice are reference dependent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 366-373.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000047URL
[39] Lempert, K. M., Johnson, E., & Phelps, E. A. (2016). Emotional arousal predicts intertemporal choice. Emotion, 16(5), 647-656.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000168URL
[40] Li, S., Bi, Y. L., Su, Y., & Rao, L. L. (2011). An additional gain can make you feel bad and an additional loss can make you feel good. Advances in Psychological Science, 19(1), 9-17.
[李纾, 毕研玲, 苏寅, 饶俪琳. (2011). 厌而避额外之“利”; 喜而趋额外之“害”. 心理科学进展, 19(1), 9-17.]
[41] Löckenhoff, C. E., Rutt, J. L., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., O'Donoghue, T., Reyna, V. F., & Ganzel, B. (2016). Dread sensitivity in decisions about real and imagined electrical shocks does not vary by age. Psychology and Aging, 31(18), 890.
doi: 10.1037/pag0000136URL
[42] Loewenstein, G. (1987). Anticipation and the valuation of delayed consumption. Economic Journal, 97(387), 666-684.
doi: 10.2307/2232929URL
[43] Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (1992). Anomalies in intertemporal choice: Evidence and an interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(2), 573-597.
doi: 10.2307/2118482URL
[44] Luce, R. D. (1959). Individual choice behavior: A theoretical analysis. New York: John Wiley and sons.
[45] Mackeigan, L. D., Larson, L. N., Draugalis, J. L. R., Bootman, J. L., & Burns, L. R. (1993). Time preference for health gains versus health losses. Pharmacoeconomics, 3(5), 374-386.
doi: 10.2165/00019053-199303050-00005URL
[46] Mamayek, C., Paternoster, R., & Loughran, T. A. (2017). Temporal discounting, present orientation, and criminal deterrence. In W. Bernasco, J.-L. van Gelder, & H. Elffers (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of offender decision making (pp. 209-227). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.
[47] Mann, K. A. (2018). Time preference and savoring -- How to exploit the Loewenstein contradiction. Retrieved February 25, 2018, from https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/id/eprint/84500.
[48] Mischel, W., Grusec, J., & Masters, J. C. (1969). Effects of expected delay time on the subjective value of rewards and punishments. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 11(4), 363-373.
[49] Mitchell, S. H., & Wilson, V. B. (2010). The subjective value of delayed and probabilistic outcomes: Outcome size matters for gains but not for losses. Behavioral Processes, 83(1), 36-40.
doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.09.003URL
[50] Myerson, J., Baumann, A. A., & Green, L. (2017). Individual differences in delay discounting: Differences are quantitative with gains, but qualitative with losses. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(2), 359-372.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v30.2URL
[51] Redelmeier, D. A., & Heller, D. N. (1993). Time preference in medical decision making and cost-effectiveness analysis. Medical Decision Making, 13(3), 212-217.
pmid: 8412549
[52] Rodzon, K., Berry, M. S., & Odum, A. L. (2011). Within- subject comparison of degree of delay discounting using titrating and fixed sequence procedures. Behavioural Processes, 86(1), 164-167.
doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.09.007URL
[53] Samuelson, P. A. (1937). A note on measurement of utility. Review of Economic Studies, 4(2), 155-161.
doi: 10.2307/2967612URL
[54] Sawicki, P., & Białek, M. (2016). Side effects in time discounting procedures: Fixed alternatives become the reference point. PLOS ONE, 11(10), Article e0165245. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165245.
[55] Schall, J. D. (2001). Neural basis of deciding, choosing and acting. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(1), 33-42.
pmid: 11253357
[56] Sellitto, M., Ciaramelli, E., Mattioli, F., & di Pellegrino, G. (2016). Reduced sensitivity to sooner reward during intertemporal decision-making following insula damage in humans. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 367.
[57] Smith, A. (1776). The wealth of nation. New York: Modern Library.
[58] Steelandt, S., Dufour, V., Broihanne, M. H., & Thierry, B. (2011). Can monkeys make investments based on maximized pay-off?. PLOS ONE, 6(3), Article e17801. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017801.
[59] Story, G. W., Vlaev, I., Dayan, P., Seymour, B., Darzi, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2015). Anticipation and choice heuristics in the dynamic consumption of pain relief. PloS Computational Biology, 11(3), 1-32.
[60] Sun, H. Y., Li, A. M., Chen, S., Zhao, D., Rao, L. L., Liang, Z. Y., & Li, S. (2015). Pain now or later: An outgrowth account of pain-minimization. PLOS ONE, 10(1), 1-17.
[61] Sun, H. L., Li, A. M., Shen, S. C., Xiong, G. X., Rao, L. L., Zheng, R., Sun, H. Y., & Li, S. (2020). Early departure, early revival: A "free from care" account of negative temporal discounting. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 16(2), 103-116.
doi: 10.5709/acp-0289-0URL
[62] Thaler, R. (1981). Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Economics Letters, 8(3), 201-207.
doi: 10.1016/0165-1765(81)90067-7URL
[63] Torrecillas, M. J. M., Rambaud, S. C., & Takahashi, T. (2018). Self-control in intertemporal choice and mediterranean dietary pattern. Frontiers in Public Health, 6, 176.
doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2018.00176URL
[64] van der Pol, M. M., & Cairns, J. A. (2000). Negative and zero time preference for health. Health Economics, 9(2), 171-175.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-1050URL
[65] Varey, C., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Experiences extended across time: Evaluation of moments and episodes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5(3), 169-185.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0771URL
[66] Xu, L., Liang, Z. Y., Wang, K., Shu, L., & Jiang, T. (2009). Neural mechanism of intertemporal choice: From discounting future gains to future losses. Brain Research, 1261, 65-74.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.12.061URL
[67] Yates, J. F., & Watts, R. A. (1975). Preferences for deferred losses. Organizational Behavior & Human Performance, 13(2), 294-306.
[68] Yoon, H., & Chapman, G. B. (2016). A closer look at the yardstick: A new discount rate measure with precision and range. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(5), 470-480.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v29.5URL




[1]李海虹, 邓州, 何欣, 申劭婧, 邹雅雯, 朱敏帆, 王芸, 谢晓非. 源于“反常”终于“常理”的禀赋效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(3): 394-405.
[2]刘欢, 隋晓阳, 黄元娜, 林榕平, 许明星. 行为经济学在养老储蓄领域的助推应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(3): 418-428.
[3]朱遥, 李建花, 庄锦英. 塞勒关于行为金融学的实证研究及其理论贡献[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(3): 439-446.
[4]王丁, 王超, 李红. 喜欢悲伤音乐的心理机制 *[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(6): 1004-1011.
[5]李晓明, 谭谱. 框架效应的应用研究及其应用技巧[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(12): 2230-2237.
[6]贾武力; 黄昌兵. 正常人与弱视患者持续闪烁抑制测量效果比较研究[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 66-.
[7]徐富明;史燕伟;李欧;张慧;李燕. 民众收入不公平感的机制与对策——基于参照依赖和损失规避双视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(5): 665-675.
[8]苑明亮;张梦圆;寇彧. 亲社会名声与亲社会行为[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(10): 1655-1662.
[9]李彬;徐富明;王伟;张慧;罗寒冰. 损失规避的产生根源[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(8): 1319-1327.
[10]高飞;张蔚蔚;潘孝富. 通货膨胀知觉的认知机制和影响因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(1): 125-134.
[11]张鸿飞;徐富明;刘腾飞;张军伟;蒋多. 赢者诅咒:心理机制、影响因素及应对策略[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(5): 664-672.
[12]马向阳;徐富明;吴修良;黄宝珍;黄雅凌. 行为决策中的忽略偏差:心理机制与影响因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(12): 1834-1841.
[13]李琦;罗劲. 如何唤起和缓解重大损失情绪—— 来自行为和脑成像的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(11): 1572-1582.
[14]黄宝珍;徐富明;王岚;马向阳;吴修良. 行为决策中的默认效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(11): 1675-1683.
[15]李纾;毕研玲;苏寅;饶俪琳. 厌而避额外之“利”;喜而趋额外之“害”
[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(1): 9-17.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=5511
相关话题/心理 科学 过程 社会 上海师范大学

  • 领限时大额优惠券,享本站正版考研考试资料!
    大额优惠券
    优惠券领取后72小时内有效,10万种最新考研考试考证类电子打印资料任你选。涵盖全国500余所院校考研专业课、200多种职业资格考试、1100多种经典教材,产品类型包含电子书、题库、全套资料以及视频,无论您是考研复习、考证刷题,还是考前冲刺等,不同类型的产品可满足您学习上的不同需求。 ...
    本站小编 Free壹佰分学习网 2022-09-19
  • 前瞻性情绪作为社会风险的信息源假说:公共场景下风险决策的情绪及文化机制探讨
    王晓田1(),王娜2,何金波11香港中文大学人文社科学院,广东深圳5181722韩山师范学院教育科学学院,广东潮州521041收稿日期:2020-12-21出版日期:2021-06-15发布日期:2021-04-25通讯作者:王晓田E-mail:xtwang@cuhk.edu.cn基金资助:国家自然 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 语言加工过程中的观点采择及其认知机制
    隋雪,史汉文,李雨桐()辽宁师范大学心理学院,大连116029收稿日期:2020-09-25出版日期:2021-06-15发布日期:2021-04-25通讯作者:李雨桐E-mail:dearliyutong@163.com基金资助:教育部人文社会科学规划基金项目(19YJA190005);国家自然科 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 面孔社会知觉中的表情效应
    李雅宁1,2,田杨阳1,吴琦1,冷海州1,蒋重清1(),杨伊生21辽宁师范大学心理学院,大连1160292内蒙古师范大学心理学院,呼和浩特010022收稿日期:2020-04-11出版日期:2021-06-15发布日期:2021-04-25通讯作者:蒋重清E-mail:jzqcjj@hotmail. ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 成熟对青春期型反社会行为的影响
    林爽,刘文(),王薇薇,张雪辽宁师范大学心理学院,大连116029收稿日期:2020-07-06出版日期:2021-06-15发布日期:2021-04-25通讯作者:刘文E-mail:wenliu703@126.com基金资助:国家社会科学基金重大项目“留守儿童社会适应促进与反社会预防研究”(19Z ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 人们如何设想未来:未来情景思维对个体心理和行为的影响
    卢蕾安,王春生,任俊()浙江师范大学教育与人类发展学院心理系,金华321004收稿日期:2020-07-09出版日期:2021-06-15发布日期:2021-04-25通讯作者:任俊E-mail:drinren@163.com基金资助:国家社会科学基金“十三五”规划教育学一般课题“基于积极心理学理念 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 姓名对个体心理与行为的实际影响:证据和理论
    包寒吴霜,蔡华俭()中国科学院心理研究所人格与社会心理研究中心,北京100101中国科学院大学心理学系,北京100049收稿日期:2020-09-02出版日期:2021-06-15发布日期:2021-04-25通讯作者:蔡华俭E-mail:caihj@psych.ac.cn基金资助:国家社会科学基金 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 傅斯年的心理学探索及其贡献
    陈彦垒(),胡志坚聊城大学教育科学学院,山东聊城252059收稿日期:2020-08-13出版日期:2021-06-15发布日期:2021-04-25通讯作者:陈彦垒E-mail:chenyanlei@lcu.edu.cn基金资助:聊城市社科规划专项(ZXYB202002019)FuSsu-nien ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 空间交互研究新视角:多重社会因素的影响
    肖承丽(),隋雨檠,肖苏衡,周仁来()南京大学社会学院心理学系,南京210023收稿日期:2020-07-03出版日期:2021-05-15发布日期:2021-03-30通讯作者:肖承丽,周仁来E-mail:xiaocl@nju.edu.cn;rlzhou@nju.edu.cn基金资助:江苏高校哲学 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 基于社交媒体数据的心理指标识别建模: 机器学习的方法
    苏悦1,2,刘明明1,3,赵楠1,刘晓倩1,朱廷劭1,2()1中国科学院心理研究所,北京1001012中国科学院大学心理学系,北京1000493联想研究院,北京100094收稿日期:2020-01-14出版日期:2021-04-15发布日期:2021-02-22基金资助:国家社科基金重点项目(17A ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 人际互动中社会学习的计算神经机制
    黎穗卿,陈新玲,翟瑜竹,张怡洁,章植鑫,封春亮()教育部脑认知与教育科学重点实验室(华南师范大学);华南师范大学心理学院;华南师范大学心理应用研究中心;华南师范大学广东省心理健康与认知科学重点实验室,广州510631收稿日期:2020-08-10出版日期:2021-04-15发布日期:2021-02 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01