删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

认知控制在第三方惩罚中的作用

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

苏彦捷(), 谢东杰, 王笑楠
北京大学心理与认知科学学院, 行为与心理健康北京市重点实验室, 北京 100871
收稿日期:2019-02-26出版日期:2019-07-26发布日期:2019-06-25
通讯作者:苏彦捷E-mail:yjsu@pku.edu.cn

基金资助:* 国家自然科学基金资助((批准号:31872782, 31571134))

The role of cognitive control in third-party punishment

SU Yanjie(), XIE Dongjie, WANG Xiaonan
School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Beijing Key Laboratory of Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China
Received:2019-02-26Online:2019-07-26Published:2019-06-25
Contact:SU Yanjie E-mail:yjsu@pku.edu.cn






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 第三方惩罚是一种特殊的利他行为, 在维护社会规范和人类合作中发挥着重要作用。现有研究对遵守社会规范行为及其认知机制进行了大量探讨, 而第三方惩罚这一维护社会规范行为的认知加工过程, 尤其是认知控制在其中发挥的具体作用, 是目前研究的热点之一。通过(1)借助不同技术方法来探究认知控制在第三方惩罚中的具体作用; (2)从发展的视角, 在认知控制发展的关键阶段来考察其作用随年龄的发展变化, 用以解释第三方惩罚这一利他行为的发展轨迹, 完善对第三方惩罚的认知基础的理解, 并构建第三方惩罚决策的心理模型。



图1研究框架图
图1研究框架图







[1] 陈思静 . (2011). 社会规范激活:第三方惩罚的心理机制及其对合作行为的影响 (博士学位论文). 浙江大学, 杭州.
[2] 陈思静, 马剑虹 . (2011). 第三方惩罚与社会规范激活——社会责任感与情绪的作用. 心理科学, 34( 3), 670-675.
[3] 丁毅, 纪婷婷, 陈旭 . (2012). 利他惩罚的发生机制及其神经基础. 心理发展与教育, 28( 6), 658-664.
[4] 黄翯青, 苏彦捷 . (2012). 共情的毕生发展:一个双过程的视角. 心理发展与教育, 28( 4), 434-441.
[5] 霍夫曼 , K.(2011). 行动中的心理学 (第八版; 苏彦捷等译). 北京: 中国人民大学出版社.
[6] 李政汉, 杨国春, 南威治, 李琦, 刘勋 . (2018). 冲突解决过程中认知控制的注意调节机制. 心理科学进展, 26( 6), 966-974.
[7] 人民日报评论部. (2018-11-14). 培育深入人心的规则意识(人民观点)——如何提升我们的社会文明 人民日报, pp. 5.
[8] 谢东杰, 苏彦捷 . (2019). 第三方惩罚的演化与认知机制. 心理科学, 42( 1), 216-222.
[9] 颜志强, 苏金龙, 苏彦捷 . (2018). 共情与同情:词源、概念和测量. 心理与行为研究, 16( 4), 433-440.
[10] 殷西乐, 李建标, 陈思宇, 刘晓丽, 郝洁 . (2019). 第三方惩罚的神经机制:来自经颅直流电刺激的证据. 心理学报, 51( 5), 571-583.
[11] Artavia-Mora L., Bedi A. S., & Rieger M . (2017). Intuitive help and punishment in the field. European Economic Review, 92, 133-145.
[12] Baumard N., Mascaro O., & Chevallier C . (2012). Preschoolers are able to take merit into account when distributing goods. Developmental Psychology, 48( 2), 492-498.
doi: 10.1037/a0026598
[13] Bellucci G., Chernyak S., Hoffman M., Deshpande G., Dal Monte O., Knutson K. M., .. Krueger F . (2017). Effective connectivity of brain regions underlying third-party punishment: Functional MRI and granger causality evidence. Social Neuroscience, 12( 2), 124-134.
[14] Bendor J., & Swistak P (2001). The evolution of norms. American Journal of Sociology, 106( 6), 1493-1545.
[15] Best J.R., &Miller P.H . (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81( 6), 1641-1660.
[16] Blake P.., & McAuliffe K , (2011). “I had so much it didn't seem fair”: Eight-year-olds reject two forms of inequity. Cognition, 120( 2), 215-224.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.04.006
[17] Blake P. R., Piovesan M., Montinari N., Warneken F., & Gino F . (2015). Prosocial norms in the classroom: The role of self-regulation in following norms of giving. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 115, 18-29.
[18] Bicchieri C., .(2006).The Grammar of society:he nature and dynamics of social norms. New York: TCambridge University Press.
[19] Buckholtz J. W., Martin J. W., Treadway M. T., Jan K., Zald D. H., Jones O., & Marois R . (2015). From blame to punishment: Disrupting prefrontal cortex activity reveals norm enforcement mechanisms. Neuron, 87( 6), 1369-1380.
[20] Camerer C. F. (2003).Behavioral game theory:Experiments in strategic interaction. New York:Princeton University Press.
[21] Carlsmith K. M., Darley J. M., & Robinson P. H . (2002). Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83( 2), 284-299.
[22] Casey B. J., Getz S., & Galvan A . (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28( 1), 62-77.
doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003
[23] Chaiken S., &Trope Y. ,(1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.
[24] Ciaramidaro A., Toppi J., Casper C., Freitag C. M., Siniatchkin M., & Astolfi L . (2018). Multiple-brain connectivity during third party punishment: An EEG hyperscanning study. Scientific Reports, 8, 6822.
[25] Corsi P. . (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). McGill University, Montreal.
[26] Crone E.A., &Steinbeis N , (2017). Neural perspectives on cognitive control development during childhood and adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21( 3), 205-215.
[27] Darley J.M., & Pittman T.S . (2003). The psychology of compensatory and retributive justice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7( 4), 324-336.
[28] Davis M.H . (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.
[29] de Waal F , (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59( 1), 279-300.
[30] Decety J., Bartal I. B., Uzefovsky F., & Knafo-Noam A . (2016). Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: Highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 371( 1686), 20150077.
[31] Decety J., & Yoder K.J . (2016). Empathy and motivation for justice: Cognitive empathy and concern, but not emotional empathy, predict sensitivity to injustice for others. Social Neuroscience, 11( 1), 1-14.
[32] Decety J. &Yoder K.J . (2017). The emerging social neuroscience of justice motivation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21( 1), 6-14.
[33] DeScioli P., & Kurzban R , (2009). Mysteries of morality. Cognition, 112( 2), 281-299.
[34] Diamond A , (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64( 1), 135-168.
[35] Egas M, Riedl A , (2008). The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275( 1637), 871-878.
[36] Eisenberg N, & Fabes, R. A . (1990). Empathy: Conceptualization, measurement, and relation to prosocial behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 14( 2), 131-149.
[37] Eisenberg N., Spinrad T. L.& Morris A., , (2013). Empathy- related responding in children. In M. Killen, & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of Moral Development (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.
[38] Enge S., Mothes H., Fleischhauer M., Reif A., & Strobel A . (2017). Genetic variation of dopamine and serotonin function modulates the feedback-related negativity during altruistic punishment. Scientific Reports, 7, 2996.
[39] Fehr E., Bernhard H., & Rockenbach B . (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454( 7208), 1079-1083.
[40] Fehr E., Fischbacher U , (2004a). Social norms and human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8( 4), 185-190.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.007
[41] Fehr E., Fischbacher U , (2004b). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25( 2), 63-87.
doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4
[42] Fehr E., & G?chter S ,(2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415( 6868), 137-140.
[43] Fehr E. Fehr E., & Rockenbach B , (2003). Detrimental effects of sanctions on human altruism. Nature, 422( 6928), 137-140.
[44] Fehr E., & Schurtenberger I , (2018). Normative foundations of human cooperation. Nature Human Behaviour, 2( 7), 458-468.
[45] Feng C., Deshpande G., Liu C., Gu R., Luo Y. J., & Krueger F . (2016). Diffusion of responsibility attenuates altruistic punishment: A functional magnetic resonance imaging effective connectivity study. Human Brain Mapping, 37( 2), 663-677.
[46] Fernández-Martín A.,& Calvo M.G . (2016). Selective orienting to pleasant versus unpleasant visual scenes. Cognition, 155, 108-112.
[47] Friehe T.,& Schildberg-H?risch H , (2018). Predicting norm enforcement: The individual and joint predictive power of economic preferences, personality, and self-control. European Journal of Law and Economics, 45( 1), 127-146.
[48] Fukushima H, & Hiraki K , (2009). Whose loss is it? Human electrophysiological correlates of non-self reward processing. Social Neuroscience, 4( 3), 261-275.
[49] Geeraerts S. B., Hessels R. S., van der Stigchel S., Huijding J., Endendijk J. J., van den Boomen C., .. Dekovi? M . (2019). Individual differences in visual attention and self-regulation: A multimethod longitudinal study from infancy to toddlerhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 180, 104-112.
[50] Glass L., Moody L., Grafman J., & Krueger F . (2016). Neural signatures of third-party punishment: Evidence from penetrating traumatic brain injury. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11( 2), 253-262.
[51] Grecucci A., Giorgetta C., van’t Wout M., Bonini N., & Sanfey A. G . (2013). Reappraising the ultimatum: An fMRI study of emotion regulation and decision making. Cerebral Cortex, 23( 2), 399-410.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs028
[52] Grocke P., Rossano F., & Tomasello M . (2015). Procedural justice in children: Preschoolers accept unequal resource distributions if the procedure provides equal opportunities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 197-210.
[53] Gromet D.M., &Darley J.M . (2009). Punishment and beyond: Achieving justice through the satisfaction of multiple goals. Law & Society Review, 43( 1), 1-38.
[54] Gummerum M.,& Chu M.T . (2014). Outcomes and intentions in children's, adolescents', and adults' second- and third-party punishment behavior. Cognition, 133( 1), 97-103.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.001
[55] Gummerum M., van Dillen L. F., van Dijk E., & López-Pérez B . (2016). Costly third-party interventions: The role of incidental anger and attention focus in punishment of the perpetrator and compensation of the victim. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 94-104.
[56] Henrich J., Ensminger J., McElreath R., Barr A., Barrett C., Bolyanatz A., .. Ziker J . (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327( 5972), 1480-1484.
[57] Henrich J., McElreath R., Barr A., Ensminger J., Barrett C., Bolyanatz A., .. Ziker J . (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312( 5781), 1767-1770.
[58] Hu Y., Strang S., & Weber B . (2015). Helping or punishing strangers: Neural correlates of altruistic decisions as third-party and of its relation to empathic concern. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 24.
[59] Jordan J. J., Hoffman M., Bloom P., & Rand D. G . (2016). Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature, 530( 7591), 473-476.
[60] Kahneman D (2011). Thinking Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
[61] Kinner V. L., Kuchinke L., Dierolf A. M., Merz C. J., Otto T., & Wolf O. T . (2017). What our eyes tell us about feelings: Tracking pupillary responses during emotion regulation processes. Psychophysiology, 54( 4), 508-518.
[62] Krueger F., & Hoffman M , (2016). The emerging neuroscience of third-party punishment. Trends in Neurosciences, 39( 8), 499-501.
[63] Kurzban R.,& DeScioli P ,(2013). Adaptationist punishment in humans. Journal of Bioeconomics, 15( 3), 269-279.
doi: 10.1007/s10818-013-9153-9
[64] Lahey J.N., & Oxley D , (2016). The power of eye tracking in economics experiments. American Economic Review, 106( 5), 309-313.
[65] Leliveld M. C., van Dijk E., & van beest I . (2012). Punishing and compensating others at your own expense: The role of empathic concern on reactions to distributive injustice. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42( 2), 135-140.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.872
[66] Li Z. Q., Wu X. Y., Zhang L. S., & Zhang Z. Y . (2017). Habitual cognitive reappraisal was negatively related to perceived immorality in the harm and fairness domains. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1805.
[67] Liu Y. J., Li L., Zheng L., & Guo X. Y . (2017). Punish the perpetrator or compensate the victim? Gain vs. loss context modulate third-party altruistic behaviors. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2066.
[68] Logan G. D., Cowan W. B., & Davis K. A . (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: A model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10( 2), 276-291.
[69] Lotz S., Baumert A., Schl?sser T., Gresser F., & Fetchenhauer D . (2011). Individual differences in third-party interventions: How justice sensitivity shapes altruistic punishment. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 4( 4), 297-313.
[70] Lotz S., Okimoto T. G., Schl?sser T., & Fetchenhauer D . (2011). Punitive versus compensatory reactions to injustice: Emotional antecedents to third-party interventions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47( 2), 477-480.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.004
[71] Lu T.Z., & McKeown S , (2018). The effects of empathy, perceived injustice and group identity on altruistic preferences: Towards compensation or punishment. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 48( 12), 683-691.
[72] McAuliffe K., Jordan J. J., & Warneken F . (2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 1-10.
[73] McAuliffe K., Raihani N. J., & Dunham Y . (2017). Children are sensitive to norms of giving. Cognition, 167, 151-159.
[74] McRae K., Gross J. J., Weber J., Robertson E. R., Sokol-Hessner P., Ray R. D., .. Ochsner K. N . (2012). The development of emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7( 1), 11-22.
[75] Mikula G., Scherer K. R., & Athenstaedt U . (1998). The role of injustice in the elicitation of differential emotional reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24( 7), 769-783.
[76] Miller E.., &Cohen J.D . (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24( 1), 167-202.
[77] Mothes H., Enge S., & Strobel A . (2016). The interplay between feedback-related negativity and individual differences in altruistic punishment: An EEG study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16( 2), 276-288.
[78] Müller-Lein? J-M., Enzi B., Flasbeck V., & Brüne M . (2018). Retaliation or selfishness? An rTMS investigation of the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in prosocial motives. Social Neuroscience, 13( 6), 701-709.
[79] Nelissen R. M.A., &Zeelenberg M ,(2009). Moral emotions as determinants of third-party punishment: Anger and guilt and the functions of altruistic sanctions. Judgment and Decision Making, 4( 7), 543-553.
[80] Overbye K., Huster R. J., Walhovd K. B., Fjell A. M., & Tamnes C. K . (2018). Development of the P300 from childhood to adulthood: A multimodal EEG and MRI study. Brain Structure and Function, 223( 9), 4337-4349.
[81] Patil I., Zanon M., Novembre G., Zangrando N., Chittaro L., & Silani G . (2018). Neuroanatomical basis of concern- based altruism in virtual environment. Neuropsychologia, 116, 34-43.
[82] Raihani N.J., &Bshary R , (2015). The reputation of punishers. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 30( 2), 98-103.
[83] Rand D. G., Greene J. D., & Nowak M. A . (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature, 489( 7416), 427-430.
doi: 10.1038/nature11467
[84] Riedl K., Jensen K., Call J., & Tomasello M . (2012). No third-party punishment in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109( 37), 14824-14829.
[85] Riggs K. J., Jolley R. P., & Simpson A . (2013). The role of inhibitory control in the development of human figure drawing in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114( 4), 537-542.
[86] Rogoff B., (2003).The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.
[87] Rosen M. L., Stern C. E., Michalka S. W., Devaney K. J., & Somers D. C . (2015). Cognitive control network contributions to memory-guided visual attention. Cerebral Cortex, 26( 5), 2059-2073.
[88] Saadeh W., Rizzo C. P., & Roberts D. G . (2002). Spanking. Clinical Pediatrics, 41( 2), 87-91.
[89] Salali G. D., Juda M., & Henrich J . (2015). Transmission and development of costly punishment in children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36( 2), 86-94.
[90] Schmitt M., Gollwitzer M., Maes J., & Arbach D . (2005). Justice sensitivity: Assessment and location in the personality space. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21, 202-211.
[91] Schmitt M. J., Neumann R., & Montada L . (1995). Dispositional sensitivity to befallen injustice. Social Justice Research, 8( 4), 385-407.
[92] Sheppes G.., . (2014). Emotion regulation choice:heory and findings. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.). Handbook of Emotion Regulation (pp. 126-139). New York: Guilford Press.
[93] Sheskin M., Nadal A., Croom A., Mayer T., Nissel J., & Bloom P . (2016). Some equalities are more equal than others: Quality equality emerges later than numerical equality. Child Development, 87( 5), 1520-1528.
[94] Shulman E. P., Smith A. R., Silva K., Icenogle G., Duell N., Chein J., & Steinberg L . (2016). The dual systems model: Review, reappraisal, and reaffirmation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 103-117.
[95] Sober E., &Wilson D. S. .(1998) .Unto others:The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior . Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
[96] Spence M.., (1974). Market Signaling:Informational Transfer in Hiring and Related Screening Processes (Harvard Economic Studies) . Cambridge, Mass:Harvard University Press.
[97] Steinberg L.., (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28( 1), 78-106.
doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002
[98] Sun L., Tan P. S., Cheng Y., Chen J. W., & Qu C . (2015). The effect of altruistic tendency on fairness in third-party punishment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 820.
[99] Tan F.F., & Xiao E. , (2018). Third-party punishment: Retribution or deterrence? Journal of Economic Psychology, 67, 34-46.
[100] Tomasello M., (2009). Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.
[101] Vaish A..,& Tomasello M. , (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 231-255.
[102] vanProoijen J.W . (2010). Retributive versus compensatory justice: Observers’ preference for punishing in response to criminal offenses. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40( 1), 72-85.
[103] Vanderhasselt M., de Raedt R., & Baeken C . (2009). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and stroop performance: Tackling the lateralization. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16( 3), 609-612.
[104] Wang J. T., Spezio M., & Camerer C. F . (2010). Pinocchio's pupil: Using eye tracking and pupil dilation to understand truth telling and deception in sender-receiver games. The American Economic Review, 100( 3), 984-1007.
[105] Warneken F., &Tomasello M . (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311( 5765), 1301-1303.
[106] Warneken F,. &Tomasello M . (2013). Parental presence and encouragement do not influence helping in young children. Infancy, 18( 3), 345-368.
doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00120.x
[107] Wechsler D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children-WISCIV. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.
[108] Wilson D.S., &Sober E. , (1994). Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17( 4), 585-608.
[109] Xie D., Pei M., & Su Y . (2019). “Favoring my playmate seems fair”: Inhibitory control and theory of mind in preschoolers’ self-disadvantaging behaviors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 184, 158-173.
[110] Yan Z. Q., Pei M., & Su Y. J . (2018). Physical cue influences children's empathy for pain: The role of attention allocation. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2378. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02378
[111] Yang F., Choi Y. J., Misch A., Yang X., & Dunham Y . (2018). In defense of the commons: Young children negatively evaluate and sanction free riders. Psychological Science, 29( 10), 1598-1611.
[112] Zaki J., &Mitchell J.P . (2013). Intuitive prosociality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22( 6), 466-470.
doi: 10.1177/0963721413492764




[1]张辉华. 社会网络视角的团队情绪智力[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1381-1395.
[2]李晓明, 邹是, 高友明. 失望情绪在不作为惯性产生中的作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1396-1401.
[3]曹思琪, 刘勋, 伍海燕. 共情可控?以自上而下视角考察共情的可调节性[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1420-1429.
[4]张珊珊, 王婧怡, 李昱汝. 情绪自旋及其心理健康功能[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1430-1437.
[5]曾宪卿, 许冰, 孙博, 叶健彤, 傅世敏. EMMN受偏差-标准刺激对类型和情绪类型影响: 来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1163-1178.
[6]王晓田, 王娜, 何金波. 前瞻性情绪作为社会风险的信息源假说:公共场景下风险决策的情绪及文化机制探讨[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 959-966.
[7]彭坚, 曹兵兵. 追随者主动工作行为的上行影响:内隐追随视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 967-977.
[8]何蔚祺, 李帅霞, 赵东方. 群体面孔情绪感知的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 761-772.
[9]尹俊婷, 王冠, 罗俊龙. 威胁对创造力的影响:认知与情绪双加工路径[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 815-826.
[10]霍超, 李祚山, 孟景. 自闭症谱系障碍个体的共情干预:扬长还是补短?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 849-863.
[11]王学思, 李静雅, 王美芳. 父母婚姻冲突对儿童发展的影响及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 875-884.
[12]丁琳洁, 李旭, 尹述飞. 工作记忆中的积极效应:情绪效价与任务相关性的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(4): 652-664.
[13]关旭旭, 王红波. 抑制引起的遗忘及其神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(4): 665-676.
[14]张衍, 王俊秀, 席居哲. 幸灾乐祸的重新审视和互动过程模型的构想[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(3): 505-519.
[15]赵小红, 童薇, 陈桃林, 吴冬梅, 张蕾, 陈正举, 方晓义, 龚启勇, 唐小蓉. 敬畏的心理模型及其认知神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(3): 520-530.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4759
相关话题/心理 科学 社会 神经 控制