华东师范大学心理与认知科学学院, 上海市心理健康与危机干预重点实验室, 上海 200062
收稿日期:
2020-09-08出版日期:
2021-08-25发布日期:
2021-06-25通讯作者:
郝宁E-mail:nhao@psy.ecnu.edu.cn基金资助:
*教育部人文社会科学研究规划基金项目(17YJA190007)The effect of anger on malevolent creativity and strategies for its emotion regulation
CHENG Rui, LU Kelong, HAO Ning()Shanghai Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Psychological Crisis Intervention, School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062, China
Received:
2020-09-08Online:
2021-08-25Published:
2021-06-25Contact:
HAO Ning E-mail:nhao@psy.ecnu.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 以两个实验考察愤怒情绪对恶意创造力表现的影响及作用路径, 并探究调节愤怒情绪对削弱恶意创造力表现的效应。实验1比较愤怒、悲伤、中性情绪下个体恶意创造力表现的差异, 发现愤怒情绪下个体生成更多、更新颖的恶意观点, 情绪唤醒度和内隐攻击性中介了愤怒对恶意创造力表现的影响。实验2探究不同情绪调节策略(认知重评、表达抑制)如何影响愤怒个体的恶意创造力表现, 发现认知重评组和表达抑制组的恶意创造力表现比无策略的控制组水平更低, 情绪唤醒度和内隐攻击性中介了两种情绪调节策略对个体恶意创造力表现的影响。上述结果表明, 愤怒情绪通过提升内隐攻击性和情绪唤醒度进而促进个体恶意创造力表现, 而认知重评和表达抑制策略可作为削弱愤怒个体的恶意创造力表现的有效策略。
图/表 10
图1实验1任务流程图
图1实验1任务流程图
表1实验1情绪操纵检验的描述统计(M ± SD)及差异检验结果
实验条件 | 前测 | 后测 | t(33) | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
愤怒情绪组 | ||||
愉悦度 | 6.18 ± 1.22 | 3.74 ± 1.58 | 8.80 | 0.000*** |
唤醒度 | 4.97 ± 1.57 | 5.91 ± 2.15 | -2.26 | 0.030* |
愤怒 | 1.56 ± 0.93 | 5.18 ± 1.83 | -10.80 | 0.000*** |
悲伤 | 2.74 ± 1.85 | 3.15 ± 1.86 | -1.19 | 0.242 |
悲伤情绪组 | ||||
愉悦度 | 5.65 ± 1.18 | 3.27 ± 1.40 | 8.41 | 0.000*** |
唤醒度 | 4.82 ± 1.31 | 3.56 ± 1.71 | 4.07 | 0.000*** |
愤怒 | 1.97 ± 1.64 | 2.62 ± 1.74 | -1.55 | 0.131 |
悲伤 | 2.03 ± 1.19 | 5.00 ± 1.81 | -7.70 | 0.000*** |
中性情绪组 | ||||
愉悦度 | 5.62 ± 1.30 | 5.38 ± 1.21 | 1.19 | 0.224 |
唤醒度 | 4.71 ± 1.36 | 4.59 ± 1.64 | 0.37 | 0.711 |
愤怒 | 1.88 ± 1.30 | 1.79 ± 1.18 | 0.46 | 0.646 |
悲伤 | 2.53 ± 1.85 | 2.62 ± 2.03 | -0.26 | 0.795 |
表1实验1情绪操纵检验的描述统计(M ± SD)及差异检验结果
实验条件 | 前测 | 后测 | t(33) | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
愤怒情绪组 | ||||
愉悦度 | 6.18 ± 1.22 | 3.74 ± 1.58 | 8.80 | 0.000*** |
唤醒度 | 4.97 ± 1.57 | 5.91 ± 2.15 | -2.26 | 0.030* |
愤怒 | 1.56 ± 0.93 | 5.18 ± 1.83 | -10.80 | 0.000*** |
悲伤 | 2.74 ± 1.85 | 3.15 ± 1.86 | -1.19 | 0.242 |
悲伤情绪组 | ||||
愉悦度 | 5.65 ± 1.18 | 3.27 ± 1.40 | 8.41 | 0.000*** |
唤醒度 | 4.82 ± 1.31 | 3.56 ± 1.71 | 4.07 | 0.000*** |
愤怒 | 1.97 ± 1.64 | 2.62 ± 1.74 | -1.55 | 0.131 |
悲伤 | 2.03 ± 1.19 | 5.00 ± 1.81 | -7.70 | 0.000*** |
中性情绪组 | ||||
愉悦度 | 5.62 ± 1.30 | 5.38 ± 1.21 | 1.19 | 0.224 |
唤醒度 | 4.71 ± 1.36 | 4.59 ± 1.64 | 0.37 | 0.711 |
愤怒 | 1.88 ± 1.30 | 1.79 ± 1.18 | 0.46 | 0.646 |
悲伤 | 2.53 ± 1.85 | 2.62 ± 2.03 | -0.26 | 0.795 |
图2实验1不同情绪条件下的恶意创造力和一般创造力表现 注: (A)MCT流畅性; (B)MCT新颖性; (C)MCT伤害性; (D)AUT流畅性; (E)AUT新颖性。误差棒代表标准误。*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001。
图2实验1不同情绪条件下的恶意创造力和一般创造力表现 注: (A)MCT流畅性; (B)MCT新颖性; (C)MCT伤害性; (D)AUT流畅性; (E)AUT新颖性。误差棒代表标准误。*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001。
图3实验1内隐攻击性在情绪与恶意创造力间的中介效应图 注: *p < 0.05, 表示在0.05水平显著; **p < 0.01, 表示在0.01水平显著; ***p < 0.001, 表示在0.001水平显著。系数为非标准化系数, ()内为标准误。
图3实验1内隐攻击性在情绪与恶意创造力间的中介效应图 注: *p < 0.05, 表示在0.05水平显著; **p < 0.01, 表示在0.01水平显著; ***p < 0.001, 表示在0.001水平显著。系数为非标准化系数, ()内为标准误。
图4实验1唤醒度在情绪与恶意创造力和一般创造力间的中介效应图 注: *p < 0.05, 表示在0.05水平显著; **p < 0.01, 表示在0.01水平显著; ***p < 0.001, 表示在0.001水平显著。系数为非标准化系数, ()内为标准误。
图4实验1唤醒度在情绪与恶意创造力和一般创造力间的中介效应图 注: *p < 0.05, 表示在0.05水平显著; **p < 0.01, 表示在0.01水平显著; ***p < 0.001, 表示在0.001水平显著。系数为非标准化系数, ()内为标准误。
图5实验2任务流程图
图5实验2任务流程图
表2实验2愤怒情绪诱发与情绪调节检验的描述统计(M ± SD)及方差分析结果
实验条件 | 前测 | 诱发后测 | 调节后测 | F(2, 78) | p | 事后比较 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
认知重评 | ||||||
愉悦度 | 6.25 ± 1.35 | 4.15 ± 1.33 | 5.93 ± 1.10 | 40.77 | 0.000*** | 前测>诱后*** 诱后<调后*** |
唤醒度 | 4.65 ± 1.75 | 5.33 ± 2.07 | 4.40 ± 1.39 | 3.50 | 0.035* | 诱后>调后* |
愤怒 | 1.63 ± 1.01 | 4.15 ± 1.72 | 1.98 ± 1.03 | 60.64 | 0.000*** | 前测<诱后*** 诱后>调后*** |
表达抑制 | ||||||
愉悦度 | 6.38 ± 1.31 | 4.25 ± 1.37 | 5.03 ± 1.39 | 42.54 | 0.000*** | 前测>诱后*** 前测>调后** 诱后<调后*** |
唤醒度 | 4.85 ± 1.31 | 5.93 ± 1.64 | 4.25 ± 1.50 | 12.23 | 0.000*** | 前测<诱后* 诱后>调后*** |
愤怒 | 1.70 ± 0.94 | 4.47 ± 1.75 | 2.88 ± 1.47 | 54.97 | 0.000*** | 前测<诱后*** 前测<调后** 诱后>调后*** |
控制组 | ||||||
愉悦度 | 6.25 ± 1.71 | 4.33 ± 1.54 | 4.28 ± 1.78 | 25.17 | 0.000*** | 前测>诱后*** 前测>调后*** |
唤醒度 | 4.72 ± 1.31 | 5.50 ± 1.95 | 5.15 ± 1.96 | 2.02 | 0.14 | |
愤怒 | 1.79 ± 1.18 | 4.35 ± 2.34 | 4.00 ± 2.92 | 24.11 | 0.000*** | 前测<诱后*** 前测<调后*** |
表2实验2愤怒情绪诱发与情绪调节检验的描述统计(M ± SD)及方差分析结果
实验条件 | 前测 | 诱发后测 | 调节后测 | F(2, 78) | p | 事后比较 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
认知重评 | ||||||
愉悦度 | 6.25 ± 1.35 | 4.15 ± 1.33 | 5.93 ± 1.10 | 40.77 | 0.000*** | 前测>诱后*** 诱后<调后*** |
唤醒度 | 4.65 ± 1.75 | 5.33 ± 2.07 | 4.40 ± 1.39 | 3.50 | 0.035* | 诱后>调后* |
愤怒 | 1.63 ± 1.01 | 4.15 ± 1.72 | 1.98 ± 1.03 | 60.64 | 0.000*** | 前测<诱后*** 诱后>调后*** |
表达抑制 | ||||||
愉悦度 | 6.38 ± 1.31 | 4.25 ± 1.37 | 5.03 ± 1.39 | 42.54 | 0.000*** | 前测>诱后*** 前测>调后** 诱后<调后*** |
唤醒度 | 4.85 ± 1.31 | 5.93 ± 1.64 | 4.25 ± 1.50 | 12.23 | 0.000*** | 前测<诱后* 诱后>调后*** |
愤怒 | 1.70 ± 0.94 | 4.47 ± 1.75 | 2.88 ± 1.47 | 54.97 | 0.000*** | 前测<诱后*** 前测<调后** 诱后>调后*** |
控制组 | ||||||
愉悦度 | 6.25 ± 1.71 | 4.33 ± 1.54 | 4.28 ± 1.78 | 25.17 | 0.000*** | 前测>诱后*** 前测>调后*** |
唤醒度 | 4.72 ± 1.31 | 5.50 ± 1.95 | 5.15 ± 1.96 | 2.02 | 0.14 | |
愤怒 | 1.79 ± 1.18 | 4.35 ± 2.34 | 4.00 ± 2.92 | 24.11 | 0.000*** | 前测<诱后*** 前测<调后*** |
图6实验2不同情绪调节策略条件下的前后测任务表现 注: (A)MCT流畅性; (B)MCT新颖性; (C)MCT伤害性。误差棒代表标准误。*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001。
图6实验2不同情绪调节策略条件下的前后测任务表现 注: (A)MCT流畅性; (B)MCT新颖性; (C)MCT伤害性。误差棒代表标准误。*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001。
图7实验2内隐攻击性在认知重评、表达抑制与恶意创造力间的中介效应图 注: *p < 0.05, 表示在0.05水平显著; **p < 0.01, 表示在0.01水平显著; ***p < 0.001, 表示在0.001水平显著。系数为非标准化系数, ()内为标准误。
图7实验2内隐攻击性在认知重评、表达抑制与恶意创造力间的中介效应图 注: *p < 0.05, 表示在0.05水平显著; **p < 0.01, 表示在0.01水平显著; ***p < 0.001, 表示在0.001水平显著。系数为非标准化系数, ()内为标准误。
图8实验2情绪唤醒度在认知重评和表达抑制与恶意创造力间的中介效应图 注: *p < 0.05, 表示在0.05水平显著; **p < 0.01, 表示在0.01水平显著; ***p < 0.001, 表示在0.001水平显著。系数为非标准化系数, ()内为标准误。
图8实验2情绪唤醒度在认知重评和表达抑制与恶意创造力间的中介效应图 注: *p < 0.05, 表示在0.05水平显著; **p < 0.01, 表示在0.01水平显著; ***p < 0.001, 表示在0.001水平显著。系数为非标准化系数, ()内为标准误。
参考文献 51
[1] | Anderson, C. A., Anderson, K. B., & Deuser, W. E. (1996). Examining an affective aggression framework: Weapon and temperature effects on aggressive thoughts, affect, and attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(4),366-376. doi: 10.1177/0146167296224004URL |
[2] | Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,27-51. pmid: 11752478 |
[3] | Baas, M., de Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta- analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134(6),779-806. doi: 10.1037/a0012815URL |
[4] | Baas, M., de Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2011). Creative production by angry people peaks early on, decreases over time, and is relatively unstructured. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(6),1107-1115. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.009URL |
[5] | Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression: A cognitive-neoassociationistic analysis. American Psychologist, 45(4),494-503. pmid: 2186678 |
[6] | Bless, H., Hamilton, D. L., & Mackie, D. M. (1992). Mood effects on the organization of person information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22(5),497-509. doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992URL |
[7] | Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition: Effects of positive and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological Science, 14(5),416-421. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01456URL |
[8] | Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1),49-59. pmid: 7962581 |
[9] | Brewer, D., Doughtie, E. B., & Lubin, B. (1980). Induction of mood and mood shift. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36(1),215-226. pmid: 7391236 |
[10] | Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. P. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3),452-459. pmid: 1403624 |
[11] | Byron, K., Khazanchi, S., & Nazarian, D. (2010). The relationship between stressors and creativity: A meta-analysis examining competing theoretical models. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1),201-212. doi: 10.1037/a0017868URL |
[12] | Carver, C. S. (2006). Approach, avoidance, and the self- regulation of affect and action. Motivation & Emotion, 30,105-110. |
[13] | Cropley, A. J. (2010). The dark side of creativity: What is it? In D. Cropley, A. Cropley, J.C. Kaufman & M. A. Runco (Eds.),The dark side of creativity (pp.1-14). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
[14] | Cropley, D. H., Kaufman, J. C., & Cropley, A. J. (2008). Malevolent creativity: A functional model of creativity in terrorism and crime. Creativity Research Journal, 20(2),105-115. doi: 10.1080/10400410802059424URL |
[15] | Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000). Dispositional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1),136-157. pmid: 10653511 |
[16] | Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2010). Implicit affective cues and attentional tuning: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5),875-893. doi: 10.1037/a0020495pmid: 20804240 |
[17] | Gilet, A. -L., & Jallais, C. (2011). Valence, arousal and word associations. Cognition and Emotion, 25(4),740-746. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.500480URL |
[18] | Gill, P., Horgan, J., Hunter, S. T., & Cushenbery, L. D. (2013). Malevolent creativity in terrorist organizations. Journal of Creative Behavior, 47(2),125-151. doi: 10.1002/jocb.28URL |
[19] | Goldin, P. R., Mcrae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63(6),577-586. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031URL |
[20] | Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1),224-237. pmid: 9457784 |
[21] | Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations.In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp.3-24). New York,NY: Guilford Press. |
[22] | Gutworth, M. B., Cushenbery, L., & Hunter, S. T. (2016). Creativity for deliberate harm: malevolent creativity and social information processing theory. Journal of Creative Behavior, 52(4),305-322. doi: 10.1002/jocb.2018.52.issue-4URL |
[23] | Hao, N., Qiao, X., Cheng, R., Lu, K., Tang, M. Y., & Runco, M. A. (2020). Approach motivational orientation enhances malevolent creativity. Acta Psychologica, 203,102985. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102985URL |
[24] | Hao, N., Tang, M. Y., Yang, J., Wang, Q. F., & Runco, M. A. (2016). A new tool to measure malevolent creativity: The malevolent creativity behavior scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 7,682. |
[25] | Hao, N., Xue, H., Yuan, H., Wang, Q., & Runco, M. A. (2017). Enhancing creativity: Proper body posture meets proper emotion. Acta Psychologica, 173,32-40. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.12.005URL |
[26] | Harris, D. J., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2015). Fast and furious: the influence of implicit aggression, premeditation, and provoking situations on malevolent creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity, and the Arts, 9(1),54-64. doi: 10.1037/a0038499URL |
[27] | Harris, D. J., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Kaufman, J. C. (2013). The effect of emotional intelligence and task type on malevolent creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics,Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3),237-244. doi: 10.1037/a0032139URL |
[28] | Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-Based approach. New York: Guilford Press. |
[29] | Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 67(3),451-470. doi: 10.1111/bmsp.2014.67.issue-3URL |
[30] | Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12),1280-1300. pmid: 9414606 |
[31] | Higgins, E. T. (2001). Promotion and prevention experiences: Relating emotions to nonemotional motivational states. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.),Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp.186-211). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. |
[32] | Higgins, E. T. (2006). Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Psychological Review, 113(3),439-460. pmid: 16802877 |
[33] | Jiang, J., Dai, B. H., Peng, D. L., Zhu, C. Z., Liu, L., Lu, C. M. (2012). Neural synchronization during face-to-face communication. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(45),16064-16069. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2926-12.2012pmid: 23136442 |
[34] | Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American Psychologist, 50(5),372-385. pmid: 7762889 |
[35] | Lu, K. L., Xue, H., Nozawa, T., & Hao, N. (2019). Cooperation makes a group be more creative. Cerebral Cortex, 29(8),3457-3470. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy215URL |
[36] | Molho, C., Tybur, J. M., Balliet, D., Güler, E., & Hofmann, W. (2017). Disgust and anger relate to different aggressive responses to moral violations. Psychological Science, 28(5),609-619. doi: 10.1177/0956797617692000URL |
[37] | Perchtold-Stefan, C. M., Fink, A., Rominger, C., & Papousek, I. (2020). Creative, antagonistic, and angry? Exploring the roots of malevolent creativity with a real-world idea generation task. The Journal of Creative Behavior. doi: 10.1002/jocb.484. |
[38] | Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Why isn’t creativity more important to educational psychologists? Potentials, pitfalls, and future directions in creativity research. Educational Psychologist, 39(2),83-96. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep3902_1URL |
[39] | Ray, R. D., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2008). All in the mind’s eye? Anger rumination and reappraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1),133-145. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.133URL |
[40] | Richetin, J., & Richardson, D. S. (2008). Automatic processes and individual differences in aggressive behavior. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13(6),423-430. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2008.06.005URL |
[41] | Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2),206-221. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.206URL |
[42] | Runco, M. A., Abdulla, A. M., Paek, S. H., Al-Jasim, F. A., & Alsuwaidi, H. N. (2016). Which test of divergent thinking is best? Creativity. Theories - Research - Applications, 3(1),4-18. doi: 10.1515/ctra-2016-0001URL |
[43] | Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2012). Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1),66-75. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2012.652929URL |
[44] | Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1),92-96. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2012.650092URL |
[45] | Runco, M. A., Plucker, J. A., & Lim, W. (2001). Development and psychometric integrity of a measure of ideational behavior. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3-4),393-400. |
[46] | Russ, S. W., & Kaugars, A. S. (2001). Emotion in children’s play and creative problem solving. Creativity Research Journal, 13(2),211-219. doi: 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1302_8URL |
[47] | Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1),145-172. pmid: 12529060 |
[48] | van Kleef, G. A., Anastasopoulou, C., & Nijstad, B. A. (2010). Can expressions of anger enhance creativity? A test of the emotions as social information (EASI) model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6),1042-1048. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.015URL |
[49] | Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6),1063-1070. pmid: 3397865 |
[50] | Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4),775-808. doi: 10.1037/a0027600URL |
[51] | Zhu, C. M., Gong, H. L., & Zheng, X. F. (2006). An experimental research on character of implicit aggression among juveniles. Psychological Exploration, 26(2),48-50. |
[ 朱婵媚, 宫火良, 郑希付.(2006). 未成年人内隐攻击性特征的实验研究. 心理学探新, 26(2),48-50.] |
相关文章 15
[1] | 李芮, 夏凌翔. 攻击动机对特质愤怒与反应性攻击关系的中介作用:一项纵向研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 788-797. |
[2] | 袁加锦, 张祎程, 陈圣栋, 罗利, 茹怡珊. 中国情绪调节词语库的初步编制与试用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 445-455. |
[3] | 宋锡妍, 程亚华, 谢周秀甜, 龚楠焰, 刘雷. 愤怒情绪对延迟折扣的影响:确定感和控制感的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 456-468. |
[4] | 莫李澄, 郭田友, 张岳瑶, 徐锋, 张丹丹. 激活右腹外侧前额叶提高抑郁症患者对社会疼痛的情绪调节能力:一项TMS研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 494-504. |
[5] | 武瑞娟, 陈玖柒, 李研. 愤怒emoji位置对发送者愤怒感知的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(10): 1133-1145. |
[6] | 华艳, 李明霞, 王巧婷, 冯彩霞, 张晶. 左侧眶额皮层在自动情绪调节下注意选择中的作用:来自经颅直流电刺激的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(9): 1048-1056. |
[7] | 孙岩, 吕娇娇, 兰帆, 张丽娜. 自我关注重评和情境关注重评情绪调节策略及对随后认知控制的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(12): 1393-1406. |
[8] | 孙岩, 薄思雨, 吕娇娇. 认知重评和表达抑制情绪调节策略的脑网络分析:来自EEG和ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 12-25. |
[9] | 孙俊才, 寻凤娇, 刘萍, 张文海. 高善良特质在情绪调节行动控制中的内隐优势[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(7): 781-794. |
[10] | 李红, 杨小光, 郑文瑜, 王超. 抑郁倾向对个体情绪调节目标的影响——来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(6): 637-647. |
[11] | 彭婉晴, 罗帏, 周仁来. 工作记忆刷新训练改善抑郁倾向大学生情绪调节能力的HRV证据[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(6): 648-661. |
[12] | 张丹丹, 陈钰, 敖翔, 孙国玉, 刘黎黎, 侯新琳, 陈玉明. 新生儿情绪性语音加工的正性偏向——来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(4): 462-470. |
[13] | 叶婉青, 李晓彤, 王大华. 老年人对夫妻间负性事件的认知性情绪调节策略及其与婚姻满意度的关系:交叉滞后分析[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(4): 426-435. |
[14] | 吕梦思, 席居哲, 罗一睿. 不同心理弹性者的日常情绪特征: 结合体验采样研究的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(7): 928-940. |
[15] | 殷融;张菲菲;王元元;臧日霞. 当抗议遭遇挫折:集体行动失利情境下情绪反应对行动意愿的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(4): 482-499. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=5027