删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

社会价值取向对自我-他人风险决策的影响及其机制

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

张银玲1, 虞祯2, 买晓琴1()
1 中国人民大学心理学系
2 中国人民大学统计学院, 北京 100872
收稿日期:2019-06-01出版日期:2020-07-25发布日期:2020-05-25
通讯作者:买晓琴E-mail:maixq@ruc.edu.cn

基金资助:* 国家自然科学基金面上项目(31771206);国家自然科学基金面上项目(31970986)

The influence of social value orientation on self-other risk decision-making and its mechanisms

ZHANG Yinling1, YU Zhen2, MAI Xiaoqin1()
1 Department of Psychology, Renmin University of China, Beijing 100872, China
2 School of Statistics, Renmin University of China, Beijing 100872, China
Received:2019-06-01Online:2020-07-25Published:2020-05-25
Contact:MAI Xiaoqin E-mail:maixq@ruc.edu.cn






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 以往关于为自己和代他人决策的冒险行为研究结果不一致, 这可能是因为以往的研究没有考虑决策情境和决策者人际特质等因素对于决策行为的影响。社会价值取向(social value orientation, SVO)是一种典型的人际特质, 是个体在对自我和他人资源分配时所表现出的社会偏好, 通常分为亲社会者和亲自我者。为探究SVO对自我-他人风险决策的影响及其机制, 采用为自己和陌生人分别完成多轮混合赌博游戏的任务。结果发现亲自我比亲社会者代他人决策更冒险。用模型量化的损失厌恶和对潜在损失的敏感度部分中介了自我-他人风险决策差异, 但只有对他人潜在损失的敏感度部分中介自我-他人决策的SVO效应。说明SVO会影响自我-他人风险决策, 且该效应可以通过对他人利益的关心程度起作用, 所以在自我-他人风险决策的研究中应将SVO这一决策者的人际特质因素考虑在内。



图1实验流程图
图1实验流程图



图2两种受益者条件下对不同潜在损失/获益值情况的参赌频率热图 注:白色表示参赌频率为1, 黑色表示参赌频率为0
图2两种受益者条件下对不同潜在损失/获益值情况的参赌频率热图 注:白色表示参赌频率为1, 黑色表示参赌频率为0



图3亲社会和亲自我者在不同情境下的参赌频率(A)、损失厌恶(B)、对潜在损益的敏感度(C)以及对他人潜在损失敏感度在亲社会者和亲自我者代他人冒险程度上的中介效应(D) 注:自己-G和自己-L分别表示为自己决策时的潜在获益值和潜在损失值, 他人-G和他人-L分别表示为他人决策时的潜在获益值和潜在损失值;a = 对他人潜在损失敏感度的SVO效应, b = 对他人潜在损失敏感度在代他人决策冒险程度上的效应, a×b = 间接效应, c = 总效应, c’ = 中介效应模型的直接效应;* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, 下同。
图3亲社会和亲自我者在不同情境下的参赌频率(A)、损失厌恶(B)、对潜在损益的敏感度(C)以及对他人潜在损失敏感度在亲社会者和亲自我者代他人冒险程度上的中介效应(D) 注:自己-G和自己-L分别表示为自己决策时的潜在获益值和潜在损失值, 他人-G和他人-L分别表示为他人决策时的潜在获益值和潜在损失值;a = 对他人潜在损失敏感度的SVO效应, b = 对他人潜在损失敏感度在代他人决策冒险程度上的效应, a×b = 间接效应, c = 总效应, c’ = 中介效应模型的直接效应;* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, 下同。



图4社会价值取向与共情(A)和他人-自我冒险程度(B)的关系 注:A中的人际反应指数(IRI)反映共情能力, B中的参赌频率差值指EV < 0时为他人和为自己决策冒险程度的差异。
图4社会价值取向与共情(A)和他人-自我冒险程度(B)的关系 注:A中的人际反应指数(IRI)反映共情能力, B中的参赌频率差值指EV < 0时为他人和为自己决策冒险程度的差异。


表1各变量的相关系数
变量 1 2 3 4
1 SVO (亲自我, 亲社会) 1
2 代他人决策L的偏回归系数 -0.273* 1
3 EV<0时代他人决策的参赌频率 -0.515*** 0.524*** 1
4 EV<0时他人与自己参赌频率差 -0.510*** 0.472*** 0.873*** 1

表1各变量的相关系数
变量 1 2 3 4
1 SVO (亲自我, 亲社会) 1
2 代他人决策L的偏回归系数 -0.273* 1
3 EV<0时代他人决策的参赌频率 -0.515*** 0.524*** 1
4 EV<0时他人与自己参赌频率差 -0.510*** 0.472*** 0.873*** 1







[1] Albrecht, K., Volz, K. G., Sutter, M., Laibson, D. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. ( 2011). What is for me is not for you: brain correlates of intertemporal choice for self and other. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6( 2), 218-225.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq046URLpmid: 20529885
[2] Andersson, O., Holm, H. J., Tyran, J. R., & Erik, W. ( 2013). Deciding for others reduces loss aversion. SSRN Electronic Journal, 62(1), 29-36. from: https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/ papers.cfm?abstract_id=2328642
[3] Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. ( 2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34( 2), 163-175.
[4] Beisswanger, A. H., Stone, E. R., Hupp, J. M., & Allgaier, L. ( 2003). Risk taking in relationships: Differences in deciding for oneself versus for a friend. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 25( 2), 121-135.
[5] Bromiley, P., & Curley, S. P. ( 1992). Individual differences in risk taking. Risk-taking behavior, 16( 6), 482-497.
[6] Canessa, N., Crespi, C., Motterlini, M., Baud-Bovy, G., Chierchia, G., Pantaleo, G., … Caooa, S. ( 2013). The functional and structural neural basis of individual differences in loss aversion. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33( 36), 14307-14317.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0497-13.2013URLpmid: 24005284
[7] Chakravarty, S., Harrison, G. W., Haruvy, E. E., & Rutstrom, E. E. ( 2011). Are you risk averse over other people’s money?. Southern Economic Journal, 77( 4), 901-913.
[8] Davis, M. H. ( 1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44( 1), 113-126.
[9] Declerck, C. H., & Bogaert, S. ( 2008). Social value orientation: Related to empathy and the ability to read the mind in the eyes. The Journal of Social Psychology, 148( 6), 711-726.
URLpmid: 19058659
[10] de Cremer, D., & van Lange, P. A. M. ( 2001). Why prosocials exhibit greater cooperation than proselfs: The roles of social responsibility and reciprocity. European Journal of Personality, 15( S1), S5-S18.
[11] de Martino, B., Camerer, C. F., & Adolphs, R. ( 2010). Amygdala damage eliminates monetary loss aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107( 8), 3788-3792.
[12] Emonds, G., Declerck, C. H., Boone, C., Vandervliet, E. J. M., & Parizel, P. M. ( 2011). Comparing the neural basis of decision making in social dilemmas of people with different social value orientations, a fMRI study. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, Economics, 4( 1), 11-24.
[13] Eriksen, K. W., & Kvaloy, O. ( 2010). Myopic investment management. Review of Finance, 14( 3), 521-542.
[14] Evans, J. S. B. T. ( 2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7( 10), 454-459.
[15] Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. G. ( 2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39( 2), 175-191.
doi: 10.3758/bf03193146URLpmid: 17695343
[16] Fermin, A. S. R., Sakagami, M., Kiyonari, T., Li, Y., Matsumoto, Y., & Yamagishi, T. ( 2016). Representation of economic preferences in the structure and function of the amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Scientific Reports, 6( 1), 20982.
[17] Fu, X. L. (2016), Emotional psychology. Shanghai: East China Normal University Press.
[ 傅小兰. (2016). 情绪心理学. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.]
[18] Genauck, A., Quester, S., Wüstenberg, T., M?rsen, C., Heinz, A., & Romanczuk-Seiferth, N. ( 2017). Reduced loss aversion in pathological gambling and alcohol dependence is associated with differential alterations in amygdala and prefrontal functioning. Scientific Reports, 7( 1), 16306.
URLpmid: 29176580
[19] Haruno, M., & Frith, C. D. ( 2010). Activity in the amygdala elicited by unfair divisions predicts social value orientation. Nature Neuroscience, 13( 2), 160-161.
doi: 10.1038/nn.2468URLpmid: 20023652
[20] Haruno, M., Kimura, M., & Frith, C. D. ( 2014). Activity in the nucleus accumbens and amygdala underlies individual differences in prosocial and individualistic economic choices. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26( 8), 1861-1870.
URLpmid: 24564471
[21] Hastie, R., & Dawes, R. M. ( 2013). 不确定世界的理性选择——判断与决策心理学 (第 2 版, 谢晓非, 李纾等译). 北京: 人民邮电出版社.
[22] Hayes, A. ( 2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. Journal of Educational Measurement, 51( 3), 335-337.
[23] Hu, X., Xu, Z., & Mai, X. ( 2017). Social value orientation modulates the processing of outcome evaluation involving others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12( 11), 1730-1739.
URLpmid: 28981906
[24] Judd, C. M., Kenny, D. A., & Mcclelland, G. H. ( 2001). Estimating and testing mediation and moderation in within-subject designs. Psychological Methods, 6( 2), 115-134.
[25] Jung, D., Sul, S., & Kim, H. ( 2013). Dissociable neural processes underlying risky decisions for self versus other. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 15.
[26] Kahneman, D., Tversky, A. ( 1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47( 2), 263-292.
[27] Kanagaretnam, K., Mestelman, S., Nainar, K., & Shehata, M. ( 2009). The impact of social value orientation and risk attitudes on trust and reciprocity. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30( 3), 368-380.
[28] Kermer, D. A., Driver-Linn, E., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. ( 2006). Loss aversion is an affective forecasting error. Psychological Science, 17( 8), 649-653.
URLpmid: 16913944
[29] Li, S. (2016). Decision-making psychology: Equate-to-differentiate. Shanghai: East China Normal University Press.
[ 李纾. (2016). 决策心理: 齐当别之道. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.]
[30] Li, Y. J., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., & Neuberg, S. L. ( 2012). Economic decision biases and fundamental motivations: How mating and self-protection alter loss aversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102( 3), 550-561.
[31] Liu, S., Gonzalez, G., & Warneken, F. ( 2018). Worth the wait: children trade off delay and reward in self- and other-benefiting decisions. Developmental Science, 22( 1), e12702.
[32] Liu, Y. F., Wang, P., Zhuang, J. Y., Zhong, J., Sun, Q. Z., & Liu, Y. ( 2014). Self-other differences in decision-making: questions, studies and reflection. Advances in Psychological Science, 22( 4), 580-587.
[ 刘永芳, 王鹏, 庄锦英, 钟俊, 孙庆洲, 刘毅. ( 2014). 自我-他人决策差异: 问题、研究与思考. 心理科学进展, 22( 4), 580-587.]
[33] Liu, Y., Li, S., Lin, W., Li, W., Yan, X., Wang, X., … Ma, Y. ( 2019). Oxytocin modulates social value representations in the amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 22( 4), 633-641.
URLpmid: 30911182
[34] Loewenstein, G. F., & O’Donoghue, T. ( 2004). Animal spirits: Affective and deliberative processes in economic behavior. SSRN Electronic Journal. from: https://papers.ssrn.com/ sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=539843
[35] Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. ( 2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127( 2), 267-286.
URLpmid: 11316014
[36] Lu, J. Y., & Shang, X. S. ( 2018). Making decisions for others: Multi-dimensional psychological mechanisms and decision feelings. Advances in Psychological Science, 26( 9), 1545-1552.
[ 陆静怡, 尚雪松. ( 2018). 为他人做决策: 多维度心理机制与决策体验. 心理科学进展, 26( 9), 1545-1552.]
[37] McClintock, C. G. ( 1978). Social values: Their definition, measurement, and development. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 12( 1), 121-137.
[38] Mengarelli, F., Moretti, L., Faralla, V., Vindras, P., & Sirigu, A. ( 2014). Economic decisions for others: an exception to loss aversion law. PLoS ONE, 9( 1), e85042.
[39] Murphy, R. O., & Ackermann, K. A. ( 2014). Social value orientation: Theoretical and measurement issues in the study of social preferences. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18( 1), 13-41.
[40] Murphy, R. O., Ackermann, K. A., & Handgraaf, M. J. J. ( 2011). Measuring social value orientation. Judgment and Decision Making, 6( 8), 771-781.
[41] Ogawa, A., Ueshima, A., Inukai, K., & Kameda, T. ( 2018). Deciding for others as a neutral party recruits risk-neutral perspective-taking: Model-based behavioral and fMRI experiments. Scientific Reports, 8( 1), 12857.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-31308-6URLpmid: 30150657
[42] Pahlke, J., Strasser, S., & Vieider, F. M. ( 2012). Risk-taking for others under accountability. Economics Letters, 114( 1), 102-105.
[43] Park, S. Q., Kahnt, T., Dogan, A., Strang, S., Fehr, E., & Tobler, P. N. ( 2017). A neural link between generosity and happiness. Nature Communications, 8, 15964.
[44] Phelps, E. A., Lempert, K. M., & Sokol-Hessner, P. ( 2014). Emotion and decision making: multiple modulatory neural circuits. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37( 1), 263-287.
[45] Pletzer, J. L., Balliet, D., Joireman, J., Kuhlman, D. M., Voelpel, S. C., & van Lange, P. A. M. ( 2018). Social value orientation, expectations, and cooperation in social dilemmas: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Personality, 32( 1), 62-83.
[46] Polman, E. ( 2012). Self-other decision making and loss aversion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 119( 2), 141-150.
[47] Qi, Y., Wu, H., & Liu, X. ( 2018). Social value orientation modulates context-based social comparison preference in the outcome evaluation: an erp study. Neuropsychologia, 122, 135-144.
[48] Qi, Y. Y., Wu, H. Y., & Liu, X. ( 2017). The influences of social value orientation on prosocial behaviors: The evidences from behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Chinese Science Bulletin, 62( 11), 1136-1144.
[ 戚艳艳, 伍海燕, 刘勋. ( 2017). 社会价值取向对亲社会行为的影响: 来自行为和神经影像学的证据. 科学通报, 62( 11), 1136-1144.]
[49] Rand, D. G., Peysakhovich, A., Kraft-Todd, G. T., Newman, G. E., Wurzbacher, O., Nowak, M. A., & Greene, J. D. ( 2014). Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation. Nature Communications, 5, 3677.
[50] Rong, X., Sun, B. H., Huang, X. Z., Cai, M. Y., & Li, W. J. ( 2010). Reliabilities and validities of Chinese version of interpersonal reactivity index. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 18( 2), 158-160.
[ 戎幸, 孙炳海, 黄小忠, 蔡旻颖, 李伟健. ( 2010). 人际反应指数量表的信度和效度研究. 中国临床心理学杂志, 18( 2), 158-160.]
[51] Shu, H., & Zhang, Y. X. ( 2008). Research methods in psychology: Experimental design and data analysis Beijing: People’s Education Press Experimental design and data analysis. Beijing: People’s Education Press.
[ 舒华, 张亚旭. (2008). 心理学研究方法:实验设计和数据分析. 北京: 人民教育出版社.]
[52] Sokol-Hessner, P., Camerer, C. F., Phelps, E. A. ( 2013). Emotion regulation reduces loss aversion and decreases amygdala responses to losses. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8( 3), 341-350.
[53] Sokol-Hessner, P., Hsu, M., Curley, N. G., Delgado, M. R., Camerer, C. F., & Phelps, E. A. ( 2009). Thinking like a trader selectively reduces individuals' loss aversion. Proceedings of the Natitonal Academy of Sciences, 106( 13), 5035-5040.
[54] Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. ( 2000). Individual differences in reasoning: implications for the rationality debate?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23( 5), 645-665.
URLpmid: 11301544
[55] Stone, E. R., Yates, A. J., & Caruthers, A. S. ( 2002). Risk taking in decision making for others versus the self. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32( 9), 1797-1824.
[56] Sul, S., Tobler, P. N., Hein, G., Leiberg, S., Jung, D., Fehr, E., & Kim, H. ( 2015). Spatial gradient in value representation along the medial prefrontal cortex reflects individual differences in prosociality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112( 25), 7851-7856.
[57] Sun, Q., Liu, Y., Zhang, H., & Lu, J. ( 2016). Increased social distance makes people more risk-neutral. The Journal of Social Psychology, 157( 4), 502-512.
URLpmid: 27685243
[58] Tom, S. M., Fox, C. R., Trepel, C., & Poldrack, R. A. ( 2007). The neural basis of loss aversion in decision-making under risk. Science, 315( 5811), 515-518.
[59] Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. ( 1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5( 4), 297-323.
[60] van Lange, P. A. M. ( 1999). The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative model of social value orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77( 2), 337-349.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.337URL
[61] van Lange, P. A. M. ( 2000). Beyond self-interest: A set of propositions relevant to interpersonal orientations. European Review of Social Psychology, 11( 1), 297-331.
[62] van Lange, P. A. M., Bekkers, R., Schuyt, T. N. M., & Vugt, M. V. ( 2007). From games to giving: Social value orientation predicts donations to noble causes. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29( 4), 375-384.
[63] van Lange, P. A. M., Otten, W., de Bruin, E. M., & Joireman, J. A. ( 1997). Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73( 4), 733-746.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.73.4.733URLpmid: 9325591
[64] van Lange, P. A. M., Schippers, M., & Balliet, D. ( 2011). Who volunteers in psychology experiments? An empirical review of prosocial motivation in volunteering. Personality and Individual Differences, 51( 3), 279-284.
[65] van Prooijen, J. W., St?hl, T., Eek, D., & van Lange, P. A. M. ( 2012). Injustice for all or just for me? Social value orientation predicts responses to own versus other’s procedures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38( 10), 1247-1258.
URLpmid: 22700243
[66] Wei, Z., Zhao, Z., & Zheng, Y. ( 2016). Moderating effects of social value orientation on the effect of social influence in prosocial decisions. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 952.
URLpmid: 27445917
[67] Wen, Z. L., & Ye, B. J. ( 2014). Analyses of mediating effects: The development of methods and models. Advances in Psychological Science, 22( 5), 731-745.
[ 温忠麟, 叶宝娟. ( 2014). 中介效应分析:方法和模型发展. 心理科学进展, 22( 5), 731-745.]
[68] Zhang, Y., Chen, S., Hu, X., & Mai, X. ( 2019). Increasing the difference in decision making for oneself and for others by stimulating the right temporoparietal junction. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 185.
URLpmid: 30787900
[69] Zhang, X., Liu, Y., Chen, X., Shang, X., & Liu, Y. ( 2017). Decisions for others are less risk-averse in the gain frame and less risk-seeking in the loss frame than decisions for the self. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1601.




[1]李琎, 孙宇, 杨子鹿, 钟毅平. 社会价值取向对自我社会奖赏加工的影响——来自ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(6): 786-800.
[2]杨玲,王斌强,耿银凤,姚东伟,曹华,张建勋,许琼英. 虚拟和真实金钱奖赏幅度对海洛因戒断者风险决策的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(4): 507-516.
[3]周蕾,李爱梅,张磊,李纾,梁竹苑. 风险决策和跨期决策的过程比较:以确定效应和即刻效应为例[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(3): 337-352.
[4]陈嘉欣;何贵兵. “金钱−环境”复合收益的风险决策:价值取向的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(4): 500-512.
[5]崔丽莹, 何幸, 罗俊龙, 黄晓娇, 曹玮佳, 陈晓梅. 道德与关系惩罚对初中生公共物品困境中合作行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(10): 1322-1333.
[6]陆青云;陶芳标;侯方丽;孙莹. 青少年应激下皮质醇应答与风险决策相关性的性别差异[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(5): 647-655.
[7]张葳;刘永芳;孙庆洲;胡启旭;刘毅. 异性交友决策任务上为不同心理距离他人决策的风险偏好[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(10): 1580-1590.
[8]徐四华;方卓;饶恒毅. 真实和虚拟金钱奖赏影响风险决策行为[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(8): 874-886.
[9]徐四华. 网络成瘾者的行为冲动性—— 来自爱荷华赌博任务的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(11): 1523-1534.
[10]贺伟,龙立荣. 薪酬体系框架与考核方式对个人绩效薪酬选择的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(10): 1198-1210.
[11]刘长江,郝芳. 不对称社会困境中社会价值取向对合作的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(04): 432-441.
[12]王沛,陈莉. 惩罚和社会价值取向对公共物品两难中人际信任与合作行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(01): 52-64.
[13]张锋,周艳艳,李鹏,沈模卫. 海洛因戒除者的行为冲动性:
基于DDT和IGT任务反应模式的探讨
[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(06): 642-653.
[14]张文慧,王晓田. 自我框架、风险认知和风险选择[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(06): 633-641.
[15]王晓田. 投资决策进化心理学的研究:预期的私人资金分配和父母对子女的差异性精力投入[J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(03): 406-414.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4744
相关话题/社会 心理 科学 实验 心理学