删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

提问类型对自传体记忆概括化的影响

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

宫火良, 杨迪(), 张方屹
河南大学教育科学学院心理与行为研究所, 开封 475004
收稿日期:2018-10-19出版日期:2019-12-25发布日期:2019-10-21
通讯作者:杨迪E-mail:yangdi0917@163.com



The influence of questioning type on the generalization of autobiographical memory

GONG Huoliang, YANG Di(), ZHANG Fangyi
Institute of Psychology and Behavior, School of Education, Henan University, Kaifeng 475004, China
Received:2018-10-19Online:2019-12-25Published:2019-10-21
Contact:YANG Di E-mail:yangdi0917@163.com






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 自传体记忆概括化程度较高是抑郁症产生与发展的重要影响因素, 探究降低自传体记忆概括化程度的方法以缓解抑郁症状、降低抑郁复发率是心理咨询与治疗中的一个重要研究课题。基于此, 本研究选取344名初中生进行实验, 实验1对比了问题状态组与正常状态组被试的自传体记忆概括化程度, 结果发现, 问题状态组被试的正性、负性自传体记忆得分均显著低于正常组, 两组在中性自传体记忆得分上差异不显著; 实验2对比了问题取向提问和解决取向提问对问题状态下人们自传体记忆概括化程度的影响, 结果发现, 解决取向提问组被试无论是在正性、负性自传体记忆得分上还是在中性自传体记忆得分上均显著高于问题取向提问组和控制组, 问题取向提问组和控制组在各类自传体记忆得分上差异均不显著; 实验3进一步考察了解决取向提问对问题状态下不同归因风格者自传体记忆概括化程度的影响, 结果发现, 内归因组被试的正性、负性自传体记忆得分均显著高于外归因组, 在中性自传体记忆得分上, 两组被试间差异不显著。结论:与抑郁症等精神障碍患者类似, 遭受一般心理困扰个体的自传体记忆概括化程度也较高; 相比问题取向提问, 解决取向提问能较好地降低一般心理困扰者的自传体记忆概括化程度; 对于一般心理困扰者而言, 解决取向提问对内归因者自传体记忆概括化程度改善效果优于外归因者。综上, 解决取向提问可以作为心理咨询师或治疗师降低个案自传体记忆概括化程度的一种有益尝试。



图1Ask-Tell模型
图1Ask-Tell模型


表1两组被试的自传体记忆测试得分(M ± SD)
记忆测试 问题状态组(n = 30) 正常组(n = 30)
正性线索词 0.53 ± 0.57 1.12 ± 0.70
负性线索词 0.47 ± 0.57 0.98 ± 0.74
中性线索词 0.43 ± 0.50 0.50 ± 0.63

表1两组被试的自传体记忆测试得分(M ± SD)
记忆测试 问题状态组(n = 30) 正常组(n = 30)
正性线索词 0.53 ± 0.57 1.12 ± 0.70
负性线索词 0.47 ± 0.57 0.98 ± 0.74
中性线索词 0.43 ± 0.50 0.50 ± 0.63



图2实验1组别和线索词的交互作用图
图2实验1组别和线索词的交互作用图


表2不同提问类型的自传体记忆测试得分(M ± SD)
记忆测试 SF组(n = 28) PF组(n = 28) 控制组(n = 28)
正性线索词 1.54 ± 0.64 0.50 ± 0.64 0.54 ± 0.58
负性线索词 1.40 ± 0.69 0.39 ± 0.50 0.46 ± 0.58
中性线索词 0.89 ± 0.69 0.54 ± 0.69 0.46 ± 0.51

表2不同提问类型的自传体记忆测试得分(M ± SD)
记忆测试 SF组(n = 28) PF组(n = 28) 控制组(n = 28)
正性线索词 1.54 ± 0.64 0.50 ± 0.64 0.54 ± 0.58
负性线索词 1.40 ± 0.69 0.39 ± 0.50 0.46 ± 0.58
中性线索词 0.89 ± 0.69 0.54 ± 0.69 0.46 ± 0.51



图3实验2组别和线索词的交互作用图
图3实验2组别和线索词的交互作用图


表3不同归因风格组自传体记忆测试得分(M ± SD)
记忆测试 内归因组(n = 25) 外归因组(n = 25)
正性线索词 1.40 ± 0.65 1.04 ± 0.61
负性线索词 1.44 ± 0.58 0.56 ± 0.58
中性线索词 0.76 ± 0.66 0.60 ± 0.71

表3不同归因风格组自传体记忆测试得分(M ± SD)
记忆测试 内归因组(n = 25) 外归因组(n = 25)
正性线索词 1.40 ± 0.65 1.04 ± 0.61
负性线索词 1.44 ± 0.58 0.56 ± 0.58
中性线索词 0.76 ± 0.66 0.60 ± 0.71



图4实验3组别和线索词的交互作用图
图4实验3组别和线索词的交互作用图







[1] Beyebach M . (2014). Change factors in solution-focused brief therapy: A review of the Salamanca studies. Journal of Systemic Therapies, 33(1), 62-77.
[2] Bower G. H . (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129-148.
[3] Conway M. A . (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261-288.
[4] Dameron M. L . (2016). Person-centered counseling and solution-focused brief therapy: an integrative model for school counselors. Journal of School Counseling, 14(10), 1-28.
[5] Disner S. G., Beevers C. G., Haigh E. A. P., & Beck A. T . (2011). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(8), 467-477.
[6] Eigenhuis E., Seldenrijk A., van Schaik A., Raes F., van Oppen P . (2017). Feasibility and effectiveness of memory specificity training in depressed outpatients: A pilot study. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 24(1), 269-277.
[7] Feng Z. Z., & Liu K. Y . (2017). Over-generalization of autobiographical memory in depression. Chongqing Medicine, 46(25), 3457-3459.
[ 冯正直, 刘可愚 . (2017). 抑郁症过度概括化自传体记忆. 重庆医学, 46(25), 3457-3459.]
[8] Giffard B., Viard A., Dayan J., Morel N., Joly F., & Eustache F . (2013). Autobiographical memory, self, and stress-related psychiatric disorders: which implications in cancer patients?. Neuropsychology Review, 23 (2), 157-168.
[9] Gong H. L., & Xu W. S . (2015). A meta-analysis on the effectiveness of solution-focused brief therapy. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 13(6), 799-803.
[ 宫火良, 许维素 . (2015). 焦点解决短期疗法应用效果的分析. 心理与行为研究, 13(6), 799-803.]
[10] Grant A. M . (2012). Making positive change: a randomized study comparing solution-focused vs. problem-focused coaching questions. Journal of Systemic Therapies, 31(2), 21-35.
[11] Grant A. M., & O’Conner S. A . (2010). The differential effects of solution-focused and problem-focused coaching questions: a pilot study with implications for practice. Industrial and Commercial Training, 42(2), 102-111.
[12] Guo Q. K., Jiang J., & Wang H. Y . (2012). Psychometrics properties of Chinese edition MMCS and the characteristics of Chinese students’ locus of control, Journal of Psychological Science, 35(6), 1491-1496.
[ 郭庆科, 姜晶, 王洪友 . (2012). MMCS的心理测量学性能及中国大学生心理控制源的特点. 心理科学, 35(6), 1491-1496.]
[13] Healing S., & Bavelas J. B . (2011). Can questions lead to change? An analogue experiment. Journal of Systemic Therapies, 30(4), 30-47.
[14] Holland A. C., & Kensinger E. A . (2010). Emotion and autobiographical memory. Physics of Life Reviews, 7(1), 88-131.
[15] Kim J. S., & Franklin C . (2015). Understanding emotional change in solution-focused brief therapy: facilitating positive emotions. Best Practices in Mental Health, 11(1), 25-41.
[16] Liang Z. H . (2005). Research for generality of autobiographical memory under different mood state and self-concept (Unpublished master’s thesis). South China Normal University. Guanghzhou.
[ 梁志恒 . (2005). 不同情绪状态和自我概念下的自传体记忆概括化研究(硕士学位论文). 华南师范大学, 广州.]
[17] Mao W. B., Mou C. C., Li D.M., & An S . (2017). Mood congruency of involuntary autobiographical memories in college students with different moods, Chinese Mental Health Journal, 31(8), 648-653.
[ 毛伟宾, 牟程程, 李冬梅, 安姝 . (2017). 不同情绪状态大学生自发自传体记忆的情绪一致性. 中国心理卫生杂志, 31(8), 648-653.]
[18] Neipp M., Beyebach M., Nunez R. M., & Martınez-Gonzalez M . (2015). The effect of solution-focused versus problem- focused questions: a replication. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 42(3), 525-535.
[19] Nolen-Hoeksema S . (2017). Abnormal Psychology. Beijing: Posts and Telecommunications Press.
[ Nolen-Hoeksema S . (2017). 变态心理学. 北京: 人民邮电出版社.]
[20] Palombo B. J., Sheldon S., & Levine B . (2018). Individual differences in autobiographical memory, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(7), 583-597.
[21] Priest S., & Gass M . (1997). An examination of "problem- solving" versus "solution-focused" facilitation styles in a corporate setting. The Journal of Experiential Education, 20(1), 34-39.
[22] Richmond C. J., Jordan S. A., Bischof G. H., & Sauer E. M . (2014). Effects of solution-focused versus problem-focused intake questions on pre-treatment change. Journal of Systemic Therapies, 33(1), 33-47.
[23] Shu L (1999). Self-rating depression scale and depression status inventory. Chinese mental health Journal,(suppl.), 194-196.
[ 舒良 . (1999). 自评抑郁量表和抑郁状态问卷. 中国心理卫生杂志, (增刊), 194-196.]
[24] Söderlund H., Moscovitch M., Kumar N., Daskalakis ZJ., Flint A., Herrmann N., & Levine B . (2014). Autobiographical episodic memory in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(1), 51-60.
[25] Sun Y . (2013). Research on the effectiveness of miracle question in solution-focused approach (Unpublished master’s thesis). Zhejiang Sci-Tech University.
[ 孙洋 . (2013). 焦点解决中奇迹提问的效用研究(硕士学位论文). 浙江理工大学.]
[26] Tetsuro O . (2002). “Problems” as resources: a practical guide to addressing clients’ description of their problems in solution-focused therapy. Journal of Systemic Therapies, 21(4), 35-47.
[27] Theeboom T., Beersma B., & van Vianen A. E. M . (2016). The differential effects of solution-focused and problem- focused coaching questions on the affect, attentional control and cognitive flexibility of undergraduate students experiencing study-related stress. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 11(5), 460-469.
[28] Tiffany L., & Neil B . (2017). Effects of a proven error on evaluations of witness testimony. Law and Human Behavior, 41(3), 314-323.
[29] Watkins P. C., Grimm D. L., Whitney A., & Brown A . (2005). Unintentional memory bias in depression. In A. V. Clark (Ed.), Mood state and health. Hauppauge, NY, US: Nova Biomedical Books.
[30] Wenzel A., Pinna K., & Rubin D. C . (2004). Autobiographical memories of anxiety-related experiences. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(3), 329-341.
[31] Whitmore J . (1992). Coaching for performance. London: Nicholas Brealey.
[32] Williams J. M., & Broadbent K . (1986). Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(2), 144-149.
[33] Williams J. M. G., Barnhofer T., Crane C., Hermans D., Raes F., Watkins E. D., & Dalgleish T . (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 122-148.
[34] Xiao J., Yan J., Hou W. Y., & Yu P . (2015). Impact of over- genenral autobiographical memory and daily hassles on depressive symptoms in college students: a longitudinal study, Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23(5), 929-936.
[ 肖晶, 闫菁, 候文颖, 于萍 . (2015). 大学生过度概括化自传体记忆与日常应激对抑郁症状影响的追踪研究. 中国临床心理学杂志, 23(5), 929-936.]
[35] Yang J., Tao R., & Chen R. J . (2013). The impacts of automatic expressive suppression on the sensitivity of emotion perception in different mood states. Journal of Psychological Science, 36(5), 1085-1092.
[ 杨静, 陶嵘, 陈锐娟 . (2013). 不同情绪状态下自动表达抑制对情绪及表情知觉敏感性的影响. 心理科学, 36(5), 1085-1092.]
[36] Yang Z. L. (1999). Psychology of memory. Shanghai: East China normal University Press.
[ 杨治良 . (1999). 记忆心理学. 华东师范大学出版社.]
[37] Zhang H.Y., & Xu Y . (2011). The standard assessment of cue words in autobiographical memory test(AMT). Psychological Exploration, 31(6), 503-507.
[ 张宏宇, 许燕 . (2011). 自传体记忆测验(AMT)中线索词的标准化评定. 心理学探新, 31(6), 503-507.]
[38] Zheng X. F., & Gong H. L . (2008) . Principles and methods of psychological consultation. Beijing: People's Education Press.
[ 郑希付, 宫火良 . (2008). 心理咨询原理与方法. 北京:
[39] Zheng X. F., Chen X. J., Huang Y. S., Dang X. J., & Huang S. S . (2012). The Effects of Emotion of the autobiographical memory on the implicit retrieval of the related information. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(11), 1463-1471.
[ 郑希付, 陈雪军, 黄月胜, 党晓娇, 黄珊珊 . (2012). 自传体记忆的情绪对其相关信息内隐提取的影响. 心理学报, 44(11), 1463-1471.]




[1]莫李澄, 郭田友, 张岳瑶, 徐锋, 张丹丹. 激活右腹外侧前额叶提高抑郁症患者对社会疼痛的情绪调节能力:一项TMS研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 494-504.
[2]孙启武, 吴才智, 于丽霞, 王巍欣, 沈国成. 阅读进度反馈信息对工作同盟和咨询效果的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(4): 349-361.
[3]侯娟, 朱英格, 方晓义. 手机成瘾与抑郁:社交焦虑和负性情绪信息注意偏向的多重中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(4): 362-373.
[4]江光荣, 李丹阳, 任志洪, 闫玉朋, 伍新春, 朱旭, 于丽霞, 夏勉, 李凤兰, 韦辉, 张衍, 赵春晓, 张琳. 中国国民心理健康素养的现状与特点[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(2): 182-198.
[5]梁一鸣, 郑昊, 刘正奎. 震后儿童创伤后应激障碍的症状网络演化[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(11): 1301-1312.
[6]韩毅初, 温恒福, 程淑华, 张淳淦, 李欣. 流动儿童歧视知觉与心理健康关系的元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(11): 1313-1326.
[7]罗一君, 牛更枫, 陈红. 生命早期环境不可预测性对过度进食的影响:基于生命史理论[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(10): 1224-1236.
[8]李世峰, 吴艺玲, 张福民, 许琼英, 周爱保. 自我肯定缓冲新冠疫情引发的焦虑反应:一项随机对照研究[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 886-894.
[9]李垚锦, 张微, 扶蓓, 周兵平. 成人注意缺陷多动障碍在内外源冲突时的表现:眼动的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(6): 777-785.
[10]任志洪, 赵春晓, 田凡, 闫玉朋, 李丹阳, 赵子仪, 谭梦鸰, 江光荣. 中国人心理健康素养干预效果的元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(4): 497-512.
[11]王文超, 伍新春. 共情对灾后青少年亲社会行为的影响:感恩、社会支持和创伤后成长的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(3): 307-316.
[12]李雄, 李祚山, 向滨洋, 孟景. 注意线索对自闭特质个体疼痛共情的影响:来自事件相关电位的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(3): 294-306.
[13]周爱保, 谢珮, 潘超超, 田喆, 谢君伟, 刘炯. 寻找丢失的自我——精神分裂症患者的自我面孔识别[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(2): 184-196.
[14]吕小康, 付春野, 汪新建. 反驳文本对患方信任和道德判断的影响与机制 *[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(10): 1171-1186.
[15]章文佩, 沈群伦, 宋锦涛, 周仁来. 基于事件相关电位(ERPs)和机器学习的考试焦虑诊断 *[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(10): 1116-1127.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4576
相关话题/心理 测试 实验 自传体 社会