删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

预期对路径整合的影响

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

张为威, 黄建平, 宛小昂()
清华大学心理学系, 北京 100084
收稿日期:2019-02-25出版日期:2019-11-25发布日期:2019-09-24
通讯作者:宛小昂E-mail:wanxa@tsinghua.edu.cn

基金资助:* 国家自然科学基金项目资助(31200758)

Influence of expectations on human path integration

ZHANG Weiwei, HUANG Jianping, WAN Xiaoang()
Department of Psychology, Tsinghua University, Beijing 100084, China
Received:2019-02-25Online:2019-11-25Published:2019-09-24
Contact:WAN Xiaoang E-mail:wanxa@tsinghua.edu.cn






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 人们通过整合自身运动信息进行的空间巡航称为路径整合。本研究采用头盔式虚拟现实和分段式虚拟迷宫, 探究个体的预期如何影响其路径整合的准确性。任务要求被试从包含4个或8个路段的外出路径的起点出发, 行经所有路段后到达外出路径的终点, 再从终点直接返回起点。三组被试在实验前接受不同的指导语, 并因此对正确返航距离产生不同的预期。实验结果表明, 个体返回起点的行为反应受到外出路径特点的影响, 而且反应的准确性在实际情形违背预期时比符合预期时更低。这样的研究结果表明, 预期作为一种非感知觉因素而影响人们进行路径整合, 体现了人类路径整合的高度适应性和策略性。



图1外出路径的平面俯瞰图示例。H代表外出路径的起点, E4和E8分别代表包含4个和8个路段的外出路径的终点, 而这两个外出路径的正确返航距离是相同的, 即C4 = C8。
图1外出路径的平面俯瞰图示例。H代表外出路径的起点, E4和E8分别代表包含4个和8个路段的外出路径的终点, 而这两个外出路径的正确返航距离是相同的, 即C4 = C8。



图2虚拟场景与路径完成任务流程示意图。(A)试次开始时被试面向第一个路段; (B)被试到达第一个路段的末端并按照提示箭头向左旋转; (C)被试行经所有路段后到达外出路径的终点, 此时迷宫墙纸改变颜色; (D)被试做出方向反应后, 一条长走廊出现在其所选方向供被试做出距离反应。
图2虚拟场景与路径完成任务流程示意图。(A)试次开始时被试面向第一个路段; (B)被试到达第一个路段的末端并按照提示箭头向左旋转; (C)被试行经所有路段后到达外出路径的终点, 此时迷宫墙纸改变颜色; (D)被试做出方向反应后, 一条长走廊出现在其所选方向供被试做出距离反应。



图3不同路段个数和正确返航距离的外出路径的平均位置误差和反应时
图3不同路段个数和正确返航距离的外出路径的平均位置误差和反应时



图4正相关信念组和负相关信念组在符合和违背预期条件下的平均位置误差和反应时
图4正相关信念组和负相关信念组在符合和违背预期条件下的平均位置误差和反应时



图5不同路段个数和正确返航距离的外出路径的平均符号距离误差和符号角度误差。
图5不同路段个数和正确返航距离的外出路径的平均符号距离误差和符号角度误差。



图6正相关信念组和负相关信念组在符合和违背预期条件下的平均符号距离误差和符号角度误差
图6正相关信念组和负相关信念组在符合和违背预期条件下的平均符号距离误差和符号角度误差







[1] Arnold A. E. G. F., Burles F., Bray S., Levy R. M.,& Iaria G . (2014). Differential neural network configuration during human path integration. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 263.
[2] Cheng K., Shettleworth S. J., Huttenlocher J., & Rieser J . (2007). Bayesian integration of spatial information. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 625-637.
[3] Chrastil E. R., Sherrill K. R., Hasselmo M. E., Stern C. E& . (2016). Which way and how far? Tracking of translation and rotation information for human path integration. Human Brain Mapping, 37(10), 3636-3655.
[4] D’Astolfo, L., & Rief, W. .(2017). Learning about expectation violation from prediction error paradigms: A meta-analysis on brain processes following a prediction error. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1253.
[5] Ellmore, T. M., & Mcnaughton, B. L . (2004). Human path integration by optic flow. Spatial Cognition & Computation, 4(3), 255-272.
[6] Garrison J., Erdeniz B., & Done J . (2013). Prediction error in reinforcement learning: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(7), 1297-1310.
[7] Guo J. C. S., Huang J. P., & Wan X. A . (2019). Influence of route decision-making and experience on human path integration. Acta Psychologica, 193, 66-72.
[8] Guo J. C. S., Huang J. P., & Wan X. A . (2019). The influence of target knowledge on path integration. Acta Psycholoica Sinica, 51(2), 188-195.
[9] [ 过继成思, 黄建平, 宛小昂 . (2019). 目标预知对路径整合的影响. 心理学报, 51(2), 188-195.]
[10] Guo, J. C. S., & Wan, X. A . (2014). Psychological and neural mechanisms of human path integration. Journal of Psychological Science, 37(2), 494-498.
[11] [ 过继成思, 宛小昂 . (2014). 人类路径整合的心理机制与神经基础. 心理科学, 37(2), 494-498.]
[12] Guo, J. C. S., & Wan X. A . (2015). The effect of learning in virtual path integration. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(6), 711-720.
[13] [ 过继成思, 宛小昂 . (2015). 虚拟路径整合的学习效应. 心理学报, 47(6), 711-720.]
[14] He, Q., & McNamara, T. P . (2018). Spatial updating strategy affects the reference frame in path integration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 1073-1079.
[15] Hovland C. I., Harvey O. J., & Sherif M . (1957). Assimilation and contrast effects in reactions to communication and attitude change. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 55(2), 244-252.
[16] Huang J., Wang F., Sui J., & Wan X . (2019). Functional and structural basis of the color-flavor incongruency effect in visual search. Neuropsychologia, 127, 66-74.
[17] Kearns M. J., Warren W. H., Duchon A. P., Tarr M. J& . (2002). Path integration from optic flow and body senses in a homing task. Perception, 31(3), 349-374.
[18] Klatzky R. L., Loomis J. M., & Golledge R. G . (1997). Encoding spatial representations through nonvisually guided locomotion: Tests of human path integration. Psychology of Learning, 37, 41-84.
[19] Klatzky R. L., Loomis J. M., Golledge R. G., Cicinelli J. G., Doherty S., & Pellegrino J. W . (1990). Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22(1), 19-43.
[20] Li, D., & Yang, Z. N . (2015). Spatial navigation: The relationship between landmark learning and path integration. Advances in Psychological Science, 23(10), 1755-1762.
[21] [ 李丹, 杨昭宁 (2015). 空间导航: 路标学习和路径整合的关系. 心理科学进展, 23(10), 1755-1762.]
[22] Loewenstein, G., & Schkade D. ,(1999). Wouldn’t it be nice? Predicting future feelings. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener E, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 85-105). New York, NY, US: Russell Sage Foundation.
[23] Loomis J. M., Klatzky R. L., Golledge R. G., Cicinelli J. G., Pellegrino J. W., & Fry P. A . (1993). Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(1), 73-91.
[24] Philbeck, J. W., & O’Leary, S. (2005). Remembered landmarks enhance the precision of path integration. Psicológica, 26(1), 7-24.
[25] Riecke B. E., van Veen, H. A. H. C.., & Bülthoff H. H . (2002). Visual homing is possible without landmarks: A path integration study in Virtual Reality. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 11(5), 443-473.
[26] Sholl, M. J . (1989). The relation between horizontality, and rod-and-frame, and vestibular navigational performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(1), 110-125.
[27] Wan, X. (2016). The phenomenon and mechanisms of path integration in humans. Hangzhou: Zhejiang University Press.
[28] [ 宛小昂 . (2016). 人类路径整合的现象与机制. 杭州: 浙江大学出版社.]
[29] Wan X., Wang R. F., & Crowell J. A . (2010). The effect of active selection in human path integration. Journal of Vision, 10(11), 25.
[30] Wan X., Wang R. F., Crowell J. A& . (2012). The effect of landmarks in human path integration. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 7-12.
[31] Wan X., Wang R. F., & Crowell J. A . (2013). Effects of basic path properties on human path integration. Spatial Cognition & Computation, 13(1), 79-101.
[32] Wang, R. F . (2016). Building a cognitive map by assembling multiple path integration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(3), 692-702.
[33] Wiener J. M., Berthoz A., & Wolbers T . (2011). Dissociable cognitive mechanisms underlying human path integration. Experimental Brain Research, 208(1), 61-71.
[34] Wiener, J. M., & Mallot, H. A . (2006). Path complexity does not impair visual path integration. Spatial Cognition & Computation, 6(4), 333-346.
[35] Wolbers T., Wiener J. M., Mallot H. A., & Büchel C . (2007). Differential recruitment of the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the human motion complex during path integration in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(35), 9408-9416.
[36] Worsley C. L., Recce M., Spiers H. J., Marley J., Polkey C. E., & Morris R. G . (2001). Path integration following temporal lobectomy in humans. Neuropsychologia, 39(5), 452-464.
[37] Zhang, T., & Li, S. G . (2011). Neural mechanism of self-motion perception. Advances in Psychological Science, 19(10), 1405-1416.
[38] [ 张弢, 李胜光 . (2011). 自身运动认知的神经机制. 心理科学进展, 19(10), 1405-1416.]
[39] Zhao, M., & Warren, W. H . (2015). How you get there from here: Interaction of visual landmarks and path integration in human navigation. Psychological Science, 26(6), 915-924.
[40] Zhou, J. S., & Zhang, K. (2005). Kinestheic, irrelevant motion and path integration. Psychological Science, 28(6), 1306-1308.
[41] [ 周佳树, 张侃 . (2005). 运动觉、布局无关运动与路径整合. 心理科学, 28(6), 1306-1308.]




[1]熊承清, 许佳颖, 马丹阳, 刘永芳. 囚徒困境博弈中对手面部表情对合作行为的影响及其作用机制[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 919-932.
[2]李俊娇, 陈伟, 胡琰健, 曹杨婧文, 郑希付. 预期错误与急性应激对不同强度恐惧记忆提取消退的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(6): 587-602.
[3]栾墨, 吴霜, 李虹. 预期交流与创造力的关系:解释水平的调节作用[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(10): 1178-1188.
[4]过继成思,黄建平,宛小昂. 目标预知对路径整合的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(2): 188-195.
[5]陈伟, 李俊娇, 曹杨婧文, 杨勇, 胡琰健, 郑希付. 预期错误在复合恐惧记忆提取消退中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(7): 739-749.
[6]袁小钧, 崔晓霞, 曹正操, 阚红, 王晓, 汪亚珉. 虚拟仿真场景中威胁性视觉刺激搜索的注意偏向效应 *[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(6): 622-636.
[7]胡月, 王斌, 马红宇, 李改. 彩民命运控制与问题购彩的关系: 基于意义维持模型的视角[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(5): 549-557.
[8]王健, 袁立伟, 张芷, 王诗忠. 视觉预期和注意指向对姿势和动作肌肉 预期和补偿姿势调节的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(7): 920-927.
[9]孙雨生;张智君;吴彬星. 上下文预期在快速场景识别中的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(5): 577-589.
[10]程垦, 林英晖. 组织支持一致性与新生代员工离职意愿: 员工幸福感的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(12): 1570-1580.
[11]周希;宛小昂;杜頔康;熊异雷;黄蔚欣. 不连续虚拟现实空间中的再定向[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(8): 924-932.
[12]崔楠;徐岚;谢雯婷. 做会后悔还是不做会后悔?——自我调节模式对不作为惯性的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(4): 423-434.
[13]李董平;周月月;赵力燕;王艳辉;孙文强. 累积生态风险与青少年网络成瘾:心理需要满足和积极结果预期的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(12): 1519-1537.
[14]过继成思;宛小昂. 虚拟路径整合的学习效应[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(6): 711-720.
[15]纪丽燕;陈宁轩;丁锦红;魏萍. 奖赏预期调节局部注意干扰效应[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(6): 721-733.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4556
相关话题/心理 科学 运动 空间 人类