

1苏州大学心理学系, 心理与行为科学研究中心, 苏州 215123)
2 辽宁师范大学心理学院, 儿童青少年健康人格评定与培养协同创新中心, 大连 116029
3 长春中医药大学招生与就业处, 长春 130117
收稿日期:
2017-12-18出版日期:
2018-11-25发布日期:
2018-09-25通讯作者:
张明,王爱君E-mail:psyzm@suda.edu.cn;ajwang@suda.edu.cn基金资助:
* 国家自然科学基金(31371025);国家自然科学基金(31600882);国家自然科学基金(31700939);江苏省基础研究计划(BK20170333);教育部人文社科基金(17YJC190024);教育部人文社科基金(17YJC190024);江苏省高校哲社科基金(2017SJB1344);中国博士后基金资助(2017M611888)The Effects of modal-based endogenous attention on sound-induced flash illusion
ZHANG Ming1(

1 Department of Psychology, Research Center for Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Soochow University, Suzhou 215000, China)
2 School of Psychology, Liaoning Collaborative Innovation Center of Children and Adolescents Healthy Personality Assessment and Cultivation, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
3 Admission and Employment Office, Changchun University of Chinese Medicine, Changchun 130117, China
Received:
2017-12-18Online:
2018-11-25Published:
2018-09-25Contact:
ZHANG Ming,WANG Aijun E-mail:psyzm@suda.edu.cn;ajwang@suda.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 声音诱发闪光错觉是指当视觉闪光伴随不相等数量的听觉声音在100 ms内相继或同时呈现时, 个体会错觉性地知觉到视觉闪光的个数与听觉声音的数量相等。研究采用经典的声音诱发闪光错觉范式, 将注意以内源性的方式指向视觉通道或者听觉通道, 考察了基于通道的内源性注意对声音诱发闪光错觉的影响。结果发现, 当注意内源性地指向视觉通道时, 相对于基线条件, 裂变错觉的量显著减小; 当注意内源性地指向听觉通道时, 相对于基线条件, 裂变错觉的量存在增大的趋势。说明了基于通道的内源性注意可以影响声音诱发闪光错觉中的裂变错觉, 但不影响融合错觉。
图/表 6

图1实验1刺激示意图 注:F1为一个视觉闪光刺激; F2为两个视觉闪光刺激; B1为一个听觉声音刺激; B2为两个听觉声音刺激; F1B1为一个视觉闪光刺激一个听觉声音刺激; F1B2为一个视觉闪光刺激两个听觉声音刺激; F2B1为两个视觉闪光刺激一个听觉声音刺激; F2B2为两个视觉闪光刺激两个听觉声音刺激。彩图见电子版。

表1三个实验各条件下的正确率平均数和标准差(%)
实验 | F1 | F1B1 | F1B2 | F2 | F2B1 | F2B2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
实验1 | 90 ± 8 | 94 ± 8 | 34 ± 24 | 89 ± 6 | 62 ± 21 | 92 ± 9 |
实验2 | 90 ± 6 | 92 ± 6 | 50 ± 22 | 87 ± 7 | 68 ± 23 | 96 ± 4 |
实验3 | 90 ± 7 | 94 ± 6 | 29 ± 26 | 88 ± 8 | 64 ± 25 | 97 ± 3 |
表1三个实验各条件下的正确率平均数和标准差(%)
实验 | F1 | F1B1 | F1B2 | F2 | F2B1 | F2B2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
实验1 | 90 ± 8 | 94 ± 8 | 34 ± 24 | 89 ± 6 | 62 ± 21 | 92 ± 9 |
实验2 | 90 ± 6 | 92 ± 6 | 50 ± 22 | 87 ± 7 | 68 ± 23 | 96 ± 4 |
实验3 | 90 ± 7 | 94 ± 6 | 29 ± 26 | 88 ± 8 | 64 ± 25 | 97 ± 3 |

图2F1B2和F2B1的正确率在三种实验间的差异比较 注:F1B2为一个视觉闪光刺激两个听觉声音刺激; F2B1为两个视觉闪光刺激一个听觉声音刺激。

表2各实验中裂变错觉的比值比率
刺激类型 | 实验1 | 实验2 | 实验3 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
裂变错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | 裂变错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | 裂变错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | |
视听刺激 | 972 | 500 | 1472 | 800 | 800 | 1600 | 1091 | 445 | 1536 |
视觉刺激 | 147 | 1325 | 1472 | 480 | 4320 | 4800 | 154 | 1382 | 1536 |
比值比率 | 17.5 | 9 | 21.95 |
表2各实验中裂变错觉的比值比率
刺激类型 | 实验1 | 实验2 | 实验3 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
裂变错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | 裂变错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | 裂变错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | |
视听刺激 | 972 | 500 | 1472 | 800 | 800 | 1600 | 1091 | 445 | 1536 |
视觉刺激 | 147 | 1325 | 1472 | 480 | 4320 | 4800 | 154 | 1382 | 1536 |
比值比率 | 17.5 | 9 | 21.95 |
表3各实验中融合错觉的比值比率
刺激类型 | 实验1 | 实验2 | 实验3 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
融合错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | 融合错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | 融合错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | |
视听刺激 | 559 | 913 | 1472 | 512 | 1088 | 1600 | 553 | 983 | 1536 |
视觉刺激 | 162 | 1310 | 1472 | 624 | 4176 | 4800 | 184 | 1352 | 1536 |
比值比率 | 4.95 | 3.15 | 4.13 |
表3各实验中融合错觉的比值比率
刺激类型 | 实验1 | 实验2 | 实验3 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
融合错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | 融合错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | 融合错觉 | 无裂变 | 总数 | |
视听刺激 | 559 | 913 | 1472 | 512 | 1088 | 1600 | 553 | 983 | 1536 |
视觉刺激 | 162 | 1310 | 1472 | 624 | 4176 | 4800 | 184 | 1352 | 1536 |
比值比率 | 4.95 | 3.15 | 4.13 |
表4各实验F1B2和F2B1条件下的反应时平均数和标准差(ms)
实验 | F1B2_R | F1B2_W | F2B1_R | F2B1_W |
---|---|---|---|---|
实验1 | 889 ± 90 | 839 ± 70 | 757 ± 83 | 762 ± 92 |
实验2 | 798 ± 95 | 710 ± 108 | 640 ± 104 | 639 ± 88 |
实验3 | 732 ± 89 | 707 ± 76 | 581 ± 86 | 613 ± 99 |
表4各实验F1B2和F2B1条件下的反应时平均数和标准差(ms)
实验 | F1B2_R | F1B2_W | F2B1_R | F2B1_W |
---|---|---|---|---|
实验1 | 889 ± 90 | 839 ± 70 | 757 ± 83 | 762 ± 92 |
实验2 | 798 ± 95 | 710 ± 108 | 640 ± 104 | 639 ± 88 |
实验3 | 732 ± 89 | 707 ± 76 | 581 ± 86 | 613 ± 99 |
参考文献 27
[1] | Abadi, R. V., & Murphy, J. S . (2014). Phenomenology of the sound-induced flash illusion. Experimental Brain Research, 232( 7), 2207-2220. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3912-2URLpmid: 24691756 |
[2] | Andersen T. S., Tiippana K., & Sams M . (2004). Factors influencing audiovisual fission and fusion illusions. Cognitive Brain Research, 21( 3), 301-308. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.06.004URL |
[3] | Cecere R., Rees G., & Romei V . (2015). Individual differences in alpha frequency drive crossmodal illusory perception. Current Biology, 25( 2), 231-235. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.034URLpmid: 4300399 |
[4] | Degerman A., Rinne T., Pekkola J., Autti T., Jaaskelainen I.P., Sams M., & Alho K . (2007). Human brain activity associated with audiovisual perception and attention. Neurolmage, 34( 4), 1683-1691. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.019URL |
[5] | Gu, J. Y., & Lv, Y. ( 2016). The different effects of selective attention and divided attention on multisensory integration. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 14( 2), 202-206. |
[ 顾吉有, 吕勇 . ( 2016). 选择性注意和分配性注意对多感觉整合的不同影响. 心理与行为研究, 14( 2), 202-206.] | |
[6] | Kamke M. R., Vieth H. E., Cottrell D., & Mattingley J. B . (2012). Parietal disruption alters audiovisual binding in the sound-induced flash illusion. Neurolmage, 62( 3), 1334-1341. |
[7] | Kumpik D. P., Roberts H. E., King A. J., & Bizley J. K . (2014). Visual sensitivity is a stronger determinant of illusory processes than auditory cue parameters in the sound-induced flash illusion. Journal of Vision, 14( 7), 60-65. |
[8] | Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9( 2), 75-82. |
[9] | Mishra J., Martínez A., & Hillyard S. A . (2010). Effect of attention on early cortical processes associated with the sound-induced extra flash illusion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22( 8), 1714-1729. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21295URL |
[10] | Mishra J., Martínez A., Sejnowski T. J., & Hillyard S. A . (2007). Early cross-modal interactions in auditory and visual cortex underlie a sound-induced visual illusion. Journal of Neuroscience, 27( 15), 4120-4131. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4912-06.2007URL |
[11] | Posner M. I., Nissen M. J., & Klein R. M . (1976). Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83( 2), 157-171. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.83.2.157URL |
[12] | Santangelo V., Fagioli S., & Macaluso E . (2010). The costs of monitoring simultaneously two sensory modalities decrease when dividing attention in space. Neurolmage, 49( 3), 2717-2727. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.061URL |
[13] | Shams L., Kamitani Y., & Shimojo S . (2000). Illusions: What you see is what you hear. Nature, 408( 6814), 788. doi: 10.1038/35048669URL |
[14] | Shams L., Kamitani Y., & Shimojo S . (2002). Visual illusion induced by sound. Cognitive Brain Research, 14( 1), 147-152. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00069-1URL |
[15] | Shams L., Ma W. J., & Beierholm U . (2005). Sound-induced flash illusion as an optimal percept. Neuroreport, 16( 17), 1923-1927. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000187634.68504.bbURLpmid: 16272880 |
[16] | Sinnett S., Spence C., & Soto-Faraco S . (2007). Visual dominance and attention: The Colavita effect revisited. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 69( 5), 673-686. |
[17] | Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: a tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73( 4), 971-995. |
[18] | Sun Y-L., Hu Z-H., Zhang R-L., Xun M-M., Liu Q., & Zhang Q-L . ( 2011). An investigation on the effect factors in the paradigm of multisensory integration. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 43( 11), 1239-1246. |
[ 孙远路, 胡中华, 张瑞玲, 寻茫茫, 刘强, 张庆林 . ( 2011). 多感觉整合测量范式中存在的影响因素探讨. 心理学报, 43 ( 11), 1239-1246.] | |
[19] | Talsma, D. (2015). Predictive coding and multisensory integration: An attentional account of the multisensory mind. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 9, 19. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2015.00019URLpmid: 4374459 |
[20] | Tang X. Y., Wu J. L., & Shen Y . (2016). The interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and exogenous attention. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 61, 208-224. |
[21] | Turatto M., Benso F., Galfano G., & Umiltá C . (2002). Nonspatial attentional shifts between audition and vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28( 3), 628-639. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.3.628URLpmid: 12075893 |
[22] | Van Erp, J. B. F. V., Philippi T. G., & Werkhoven P . (2013). Observers can reliably identify illusory flashes in the illusory flash paradigm. Experimental Brain Research, 226( 1), 73-79. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3413-8URL |
[23] | Watkins S., Shams L., Josephs O., & Rees G . (2007). Activity in human V1 follows multisensory perception. NeuroImage, 37( 2), 572-578. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.027URL |
[24] | Watkins S., Shams L., Tanaka S., Haynes J.-D., & Rees G . (2006). Sound alters activity in human V1 in association with illusory visual perception. NeuroImage, 31( 3), 1247-1256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.016URL |
[25] | Wozny D. R., Beierholm U. R., & Shams L . (2008). Human trimodal perception follows optimal statistical inference. Journal of Vision, 8( 3), 1-11. |
[26] | Whittingham K. M., Mcdonald J. S ., & Clifford, C. W. G. (2014). Synesthetes show normal sound-induced flash fission and fusion illusions. Vision Research, 105, 1-9. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.08.010URLpmid: 25173429 |
[27] | Yu W., Wang A. J., & Zhang M . ( 2017). Effect of selective and divided attentions on auditory dominance in multisensory integration. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49( 2), 164-173. |
[ 于薇, 王爱君, 张明 . ( 2017). 集中和分散注意对多感觉整合中听觉主导效应的影响. 心理学报, 49( 2), 164-173.] |
相关文章 5
[1] | 周衡, 何华, 于薇, 王爱君, 张明. 老年人声音诱发闪光错觉的大脑静息态低频振幅[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 823-834. |
[2] | 于薇;王爱君;张明. 集中和分散注意对多感觉整合中听觉主导效应的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(2): 164-173. |
[3] | 赵亚军,张智君. 眼睛注视线索提示效应: 内源性注意还是外源性注意?[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(12): 1133-1142. |
[4] | 刘超,买晓琴,傅小兰. 内源性注意与外源性注意对数字加工的不同影响[J]. 心理学报, 2005, 37(02): 167-177. |
[5] | 刘超,买晓琴,傅小兰. 不同注意条件下的空间-数字反应编码联合效应[J]. 心理学报, 2004, 36(06): 671-680. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4297