删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

社会互动加工的认知特性及脑机制——第三人称的视角

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

程羽慧, 袁祥勇(), 蒋毅
中国科学院心理研究所, 脑与认知科学国家重点实验室, 脑科学与智能技术卓越创新中心, 北京 100101;中国科学院大学心理学系, 北京 100049;北京脑科学与类脑研究中心, 北京 102206
收稿日期:2020-05-14出版日期:2021-03-15发布日期:2021-01-26
通讯作者:袁祥勇E-mail:yuanxy@psych.ac.cn

基金资助:国家自然科学基金(31830037);国家自然科学基金(31525011);中国科学院战略性先导科技专项(XDB32010300);中国科学院前沿科学重点研究计划项目(QYZDB-SSW-SMC030);中央高校基本科研业务费专项资金资助

The cognitive characteristics of and the brain mechanisms underlying social interaction processing from a third-person perspective

CHENG Yuhui, YUAN Xiangyong(), JIANG Yi
State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China;Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China;Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 102206, China
Received:2020-05-14Online:2021-03-15Published:2021-01-26
Contact:YUAN Xiangyong E-mail:yuanxy@psych.ac.cn






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 在日常生活中, 从“第三人称”的视角识别并理解他人的社会互动至关重要。这种社会互动加工具有两种认知特性:构形整体性和动作关联性; 体现为一个由众多脑区共同参与的层级加工过程, 主要包括个体知觉网络、动作观察网络和心智化网络。其中, 后颞上沟等脑区在表征社会互动关系中起着关键作用。未来的研究需要结合多种技术手段进一步揭示社会互动加工的遗传特性和神经机制, 并关注它在实际生活中的应用。



图1社会互动加工过程的脑网络。各个脑区分别对应着下方相同颜色的脑网络。
图1社会互动加工过程的脑网络。各个脑区分别对应着下方相同颜色的脑网络。







[1] Abassi, E., & Papeo, L. (2020). The representation of two-body shapes in the human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 40(4), 852-863.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1378-19.2019URLpmid: 31801812
[2] Arioli, M., & Canessa, N. (2019). Neural processing of social interaction: Coordinate-based meta-analytic evidence from human neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 40(13), 3712-3737.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.24627URLpmid: 31077492
[3] Arioli, M., Perani, D., Cappa, S., Proverbio, A. M., Zani, A., Falini, A., & Canessa, N. (2018). Affective and cooperative social interactions modulate effective connectivity within and between the mirror and mentalizing systems. Human Brain Mapping, 39(3), 1412-1427.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.23930URLpmid: 29265483
[4] Canessa, N., Alemanno, F., Riva, F., Zani, A., Proverbio, A. M., Mannara, N., ... Cappa, S. F. (2012). The neural bases of social intention understanding: The role of interaction goals. PLoS ONE, 7(7), e42347.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042347URLpmid: 22848759
[5] Capozzi, F., & Ristic, J. (2018). How attention gates social interactions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1426(1), 179-198.
doi: 10.1111/nyas.2018.1426.issue-1URL
[6] Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1148-1167.
URLpmid: 20056149
[7] Centelles, L., Assaiante, C., Etchegoyhen, K., Bouvard, M., & Schmitz, C. (2013). From action to interaction: Exploring the contribution of body motion cues to social understanding in typical development and in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(5), 1140-1150.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1655-0URL
[8] Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interperting the expressive behavior of others: The interpersonal perception task. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13(4), 225-245.
doi: 10.1007/BF00990295URL
[9] Deen, B., Koldewyn, K., Kanwisher, N., & Saxe, R. (2015). Functional organization of social perception and cognition in the superior temporal sulcus. Cerebral Cortex, 25(11), 4596-4609.
URLpmid: 26048954
[10] Ding, X. W., Gao, Z. F., & Shen, M. W. (2017). Two equals one: Two human actions during social interaction are grouped as one unit in working memory. Psychological Science, 28(9), 1311-1320.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617707318URLpmid: 28719763
[11] Downing, P. E., Peelen, M. V., Wiggett, A. J., & Tew, B. D. (2006). The role of the extrastriate body area in action perception. Social Neuroscience, 1(1), 52-62.
doi: 10.1080/17470910600668854URLpmid: 18633775
[12] Farah, M. J., Tanaka, J. W., & Drain, H. M. (1995). What causes the face inversion effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 628-634.
doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.21.3.628URLpmid: 7790837
[13] Fedorov, L. A., Chang, D. S., Giese, M. A., Bulthoff, H. H., & de la Rosa, S. (2018). Adaptation aftereffects reveal representations for encoding of contingent social actions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(29), 7515-7520.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1801364115URL
[14] Floyd, K., & Erbert, L. A. (2003). Relational message interpretations of nonverbal matching behavior: An application of the social meaning model. The Journal of Social Psychology, 143(5), 581-597.
doi: 10.1080/00224540309598465URLpmid: 14609054
[15] Galazka, M., & Nystr?m, P. (2016). Infants’ preference for individual agents within chasing interactions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 53-70.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.010URLpmid: 27017143
[16] Galazka, M. A., Roche, L., Nystrom, P., & Falck-Ytter, T. (2014). Human infants detect other people's interactions based on complex patterns of kinematic information. PLoS ONE, 9(11), e112432.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112432URLpmid: 25409449
[17] Gao, T., Scholl, B. J., & McCarthy, G. (2012). Dissociating the detection of intentionality from animacy in the right posterior superior temporal sulcus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(41), 14276-14280.
URLpmid: 23055497
[18] Georgescu, A. L., Kuzmanovic, B., Santos, N. S., Tepest, R., Bente, G., Tittgemeyer, M., & Vogeley, K. (2014). Perceiving nonverbal behavior: Neural correlates of processing movement fluency and contingency in dyadic interactions. Human Brain Mapping, 35(4), 1362-1378.
URLpmid: 23813661
[19] Goffaux, V., Peters, J. C., Haubrechts, J., Schiltz, C., Jansma, B., & Goebel, R. (2011). From coarse to fine? Spatial and temporal dynamics of cortical face processing. Cerebral Cortex, 21(2), 467-476.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq112URLpmid: 20576927
[20] Hartmann, M., Mavrolampados, A., Allingham, E., Carlson, E., Burger, B., & Toiviainen, P. (2019). Kinematics of perceived dyadic coordination in dance. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 15594.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-52097-6URLpmid: 31666586
[21] Hauser, M., & Wood, J. (2010). Evolving the capacity to understand actions, intentions, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 61(1), 303-324.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100434URL
[22] Isik, L., Koldewyn, K., Beeler, D., & Kanwisher, N. (2017). Perceiving social interactions in the posterior superior temporal sulcus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(43), E9145-E9152.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1714471114URL
[23] Ji, H. C., Yin, J., Huang, Y. S., & Ding, X. W. (2020). Selective attention operates on the group level for interactive biological motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 46(12), 1434-1442.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000866URL
[24] Jiang, J., Dai, B. H., Peng, D. L., Zhu, C. Z., Liu, L., & Lu, C. M. (2012). Neural synchronization during face-to-face communication. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(45), 16064-16069.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2926-12.2012URL
[25] Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2007). Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 194-196.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.002URLpmid: 17344090
[26] Kingsbury, L., & Hong, W. (2020). A multi-brain framework for social interaction. Trends in neurosciences, 43(9), 651-666.
URLpmid: 32709376
[27] Kuschefski, M., Falter-Wagner, C. M., Bente, G., Vogeley, K., & Georgescu, A. L. (2019). Inferring power and dominance from dyadic nonverbal interactions in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 12(3), 505-516.
URLpmid: 30629333
[28] Lahnakoski, J. M., Glerean, E., Salmi, J., Jaaskelainen, I. P., Sams, M., Hari, R., & Nummenmaa, L. (2012). Naturalistic fMRI mapping reveals superior temporal sulcus as the hub for the distributed brain network for social perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(8), 233.
[29] Liu, R., Yuan, X. Y., Chen, K. P., Jiang, Y., & Zhou, W. (2018). Perception of social interaction compresses subjective duration in an oxytocin-dependent manner. Elife, 7, e32100.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.32100URLpmid: 29784084
[30] Manera, V., Becchio, C., Schouten, B., Bara, B. G., & Verfaillie, K. (2011). Communicative interactions improve visual detection of biological motion. PLoS ONE, 6(1), e14594.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014594URLpmid: 21297865
[31] Manera, V., Giudice, M. D., Bara, B. G., Verfaillie, K., & Becchio, C. (2011). The second-agent effect: Communicative gestures increase the likelihood of perceiving a second agent. PLoS ONE, 6(7), e22650.
URLpmid: 21829472
[32] Manera, V., Iani, F., Bourgeois, J., Haman, M., Okruszek, L. P., Rivera, S. M., ... Becchio, C. (2015). The Multilingual CID-5: A new tool to study the perception of communicative interactions in different languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1724.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01724URLpmid: 26635651
[33] Manera, V., Schouten, B., Becchio, C., Bara, B. G., & Verfaillie, K. (2010). Inferring intentions from biological motion: A stimulus set of point-light communicative interactions. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 168-178.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.168URL
[34] Manera, V., Schouten, B., Verfaillie, K., & Becchio, C. (2013). Time will show: Real time predictions during interpersonal action perception. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e54949.
URLpmid: 23349992
[35] Manera, V., von der Lühe, T., Schilbach, L., Verfaillie, K., & Becchio, C. (2016). Communicative interactions in point-light displays: Choosing among multiple response alternatives. Behavior Research Methods, 48(4), 1580-1590.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0669-xURLpmid: 26487054
[36] Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2009). Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 320-339.
URLpmid: 25164936
[37] Mast, M. S., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 145-165.
doi: 10.1023/B:JONB.0000039647.94190.21URL
[38] Matheson, H., Moore, C., & Akhtar, N. (2013). The development of social learning in interactive and observational contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 161-172.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.003URLpmid: 23164286
[39] Milinski, M. (2016). Reputation, a universal currency for human social interactions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1687), 20150100.
[40] Neri, P., Luu, J. Y., & Levi, D. M. (2006). Meaningful interactions can enhance visual discrimination of human agents. Nature Neuroscience, 9(9), 1186-1192.
doi: 10.1038/nn1759URLpmid: 16936721
[41] Okruszek, L., Haman, M., Kalinowski, K., Talarowska, M., Becchio, C., & Manera, V. (2015). Impaired recognition of communicative interactions from biological motion in schizophrenia. PLoS ONE, 10(2), e0116793.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116793URLpmid: 25664584
[42] Okruszek, L., Piejka, A., Wysokinski, A., Szczepocka, E., & Manera, V. (2018). Biological motion sensitivity, but not interpersonal predictive coding is impaired in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(3), 305-313.
doi: 10.1037/abn0000335URLpmid: 29369645
[43] Okruszek, L., Piejka, A., Wysokiński, A., Szczepocka, E., & Manera, V. (2019). The second agent effect: Interpersonal predictive coding in people with schizophrenia. Social Neuroscience, 14(2), 208-213.
doi: 10.1080/17470919.2017.1415969URLpmid: 29227757
[44] Papeo, L., & Abassi, E. (2019). Seeing social events: The visual specialization for dyadic human-human interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(7), 877-888.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000646URLpmid: 30998069
[45] Papeo, L., Goupil, N., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2019). Visual search for people among people. Psychological Science, 30(10), 1483-1496.
URLpmid: 31532709
[46] Papeo, L., Stein, T., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2017). The two-body inversion effect. Psychological Science, 28(3), 369-379.
URLpmid: 28140764
[47] Peng, Y. J., Ichien, N., & Lu, H. J. (2020). Causal actions enhance perception of continuous body movements. Cognition, 194, 104060.
URLpmid: 31518908
[48] Pesquita, A., Whitwell, R. L., & Enns, J. T. (2018). Predictive joint-action model: A hierarchical predictive approach to human cooperation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(5), 1751-1769.
URLpmid: 29119405
[49] Quadflieg, S., Gentile, F., & Rossion, B. (2015). The neural basis of perceiving person interactions. Cortex, 70, 5-20.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.12.020URLpmid: 25697049
[50] Quadflieg, S., & Koldewyn, K. (2017). The neuroscience of people watching: How the human brain makes sense of other people's encounters. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1396(1), 166-182.
URLpmid: 28405964
[51] Reed, C. L., Stone, V. E., Bozova, S., & Tanaka, J. (2003). The body-inversion effect. Psychological Science, 14(4), 302-308.
URLpmid: 12807401
[52] Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second- person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 393-414.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000660URL
[53] Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 9-34.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009URLpmid: 24486722
[54] Shen, M. W., Yin, J., Ding, X. W., Shui, R., & Zhou, J. F. (2016). Deployment of attention on handshakes. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 681.
URLpmid: 27242595
[55] Sinke, C. B. A., Sorger, B., Goebel, R., & de Gelder, B. (2010). Tease or threat? Judging social interactions from bodily expressions. Neuroimage, 49(2), 1717-1727.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.065URL
[56] Sliwa, J., & Freiwald, W. A. (2017). A dedicated network for social interaction processing in the primate brain. Science, 356(6339), 745-745.
doi: 10.1126/science.aam6383URLpmid: 28522533
[57] Su, J. Z., van Boxtel, J. J. A., & Lu, H. J. (2016). Social interactions receive priority to conscious perception. PLoS ONE, 11(8), e0160468.
URLpmid: 27509028
[58] Vestner, T., Gray, K. L. H., & Cook, R. (2020). Why are social interactions found quickly in visual search tasks? Cognition, 200, 104270.
URLpmid: 32220782
[59] Vestner, T., Tipper, S. P., Hartley, T., Over, H., & Rueschemeyer, S. A. (2019). Bound together: Social binding leads to faster processing, spatial distortion, and enhanced memory of interacting partners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(7), 1251-1268.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000545URL
[60] von der Luhe, T., Manera, V., Barisic, I., Becchio, C., Vogeley, K., & Schilbach, L. (2016). Interpersonal predictive coding, not action perception, is impaired in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 371(1693), 20150373.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0373URL
[61] Walbrin, J., Downing, P., & Koldewyn, K. (2018). Neural responses to visually observed social interactions. Neuropsychologia, 112, 31-39.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.023URLpmid: 29476765
[62] Walbrin, J., & Koldewyn, K. (2019). Dyadic interaction processing in the posterior temporal cortex. Neuroimage, 198, 296-302.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.027URLpmid: 31100434
[63] Walter, H., Ciaramidaro, A., Adenzato, M., Vasic, N., Ardito, R. B., Erk, S., & Bara, B. G. (2009). Dysfunction of the social brain in schizophrenia is modulated by intention type: An fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 166-176.
URLpmid: 19287044
[64] Wang, Y., Wang, L., Xu, Q., Liu, D., Chen, L. H., Troje, N. F., ... Jiang, Y. (2018). Heritable aspects of biological motion perception and its covariation with autistic traits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(8), 1937-1942.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1714655115URL
[65] Wu, X. Y., Hua, R., Yang, Z. X., & Yin, J. (2018). The influence of intention and outcome on evaluations of social interaction. Acta Psychologica, 182, 75-81.
URLpmid: 29149691
[66] Yang, D. Y.-J., Rosenblau, G., Keifer, C., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2015). An integrative neural model of social perception, action observation, and theory of mind. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 263-275.
URLpmid: 25660957
[67] Yin, J., Ding, X. W., Zhou, J. F., Shui, R., Li, X. Y., & Shen, M. W. (2013). Social grouping: Perceptual grouping of objects by cooperative but not competitive relationships in dynamic chase. Cognition, 129(1), 194-204.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.013URL
[68] Yin, J., Xu, H. K., Duan, J. P., & Shen, M. W. (2018). Object-based attention on social units: Visual selection of hands performing a social interaction. Psychological Science, 29(7), 1040-1048.
URLpmid: 29741989
[69] Zaini, H., Fawcett, J. M., White, N. C., & Newman, A. J. (2013). Communicative and noncommunicative point-light actions featuring high-resolution representation of the hands and fingers. Behavior Research Methods, 45(2), 319-328.
URLpmid: 23073730
[70] Zhou, C., Han, M., Liang, Q., Hu, Y. F., & Kuai, S. G. (2019). A social interaction field model accurately identifies static and dynamic social groupings. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(8), 847-855.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0618-2URLpmid: 31182793
[71] Zillekens, I. C., Brandi, M.-L., Lahnakoski, J. M., Koul, A., Manera, V., Becchio, C., & Schilbach, L. (2019). Increased functional coupling of the left amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during the perception of communicative point-light stimuli. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(1), 97-107.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy105URLpmid: 30481356




[1]卫垌圻, 曹慧, 毕鸿燕, 杨炀. 发展性阅读障碍书写加工缺陷及其神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(1): 75-84.
[2]郭容, 傅鑫媛. 社会阶层信号及其对人际水平社会互动的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(7): 1268-1274.
[3]杜碧煊, 张明明, 张珂烨, 任杰, 何蔚祺. 肢体形状和肢体动作表征的早期发展[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(10): 1703-1712.
[4]张珂烨, 张明明, 刘田田, 罗文波, 何蔚祺. 肢体识别的倒置效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(1): 27-36.
[5]尹述飞, 李添, 朱心怡. 主观记忆减退老年人情节记忆的行为表现及其脑机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(1): 51-59.
[6]刘迪迪, 王美萍, 陈翩, 张文新. COMT基因Val158Met多态性与抑郁的关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(8): 1429-1437.
[7]孙岩, 房林, 王亭予, 崔丽. 自闭症谱系障碍者抑制控制的影响因素及神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(8): 1450-1464.
[8]赵书松, 张一杰, 赵君. 第三方组织公平:研究视角、内容与设计[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(12): 2216-2229.
[9]刘立敏, 田相娟, 张文新, 王美萍. MAOA基因与环境对反社会行为的交互作用 及其可能的脑机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(6): 970-979.
[10]钱浩悦;朱敏;高湘萍. 老年人面孔构形加工特点[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(2): 230-236.
[11]李琎; 范伟; 张文洁; 钟毅平. 旋转自我面孔识别及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 50-.
[12]岳童;黄希庭. 共情特质的神经生物学基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(9): 1368-1376.
[13]任俊, 朱琼嫦. 内隐情绪调节:一种有效的自我保护[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(12): 2054-2063.
[14]孙泽坤, 王锦琰, 罗非. 疼痛与注意的交互作用:自下而上的捕获效应#br# 和自上而下的调节作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(12): 2096-2106.
[15]刘超. 人类的笑:进化论与社会互动论的视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(11): 2020-2028.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=5382
相关话题/心理 科学 社会 网络 过程

  • 领限时大额优惠券,享本站正版考研考试资料!
    大额优惠券
    优惠券领取后72小时内有效,10万种最新考研考试考证类电子打印资料任你选。涵盖全国500余所院校考研专业课、200多种职业资格考试、1100多种经典教材,产品类型包含电子书、题库、全套资料以及视频,无论您是考研复习、考证刷题,还是考前冲刺等,不同类型的产品可满足您学习上的不同需求。 ...
    本站小编 Free壹佰分学习网 2022-09-19
  • 幸灾乐祸的重新审视和互动过程模型的构想
    张衍1,王俊秀1,席居哲2,3()1中国社会科学院社会学所,北京1007322华东师范大学心理与认知科学学院,上海市心理健康与危机干预重点实验室,上海2000623华东师范大学附属精神卫生中心,上海200335收稿日期:2020-07-28出版日期:2021-03-15发布日期:2021-01-26 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 恐惧管理理论的争议及其对死亡心理研究的启示
    孟祥寒1,李强1,2(),周彦榜1,王进31南开大学社会心理学系,天津3003502南开大学滨海学院,天津3002703天津职业技术师范大学,天津300222收稿日期:2020-02-29出版日期:2021-03-15发布日期:2021-01-26通讯作者:李强E-mail:liqiangp@126 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 亲社会行为中的从众效应
    魏真瑜1(),邓湘树1,赵治瀛21西南财经大学社会发展研究院,成都6111302耶鲁大学医学院,美国收稿日期:2020-03-31出版日期:2021-03-15发布日期:2021-01-26通讯作者:魏真瑜E-mail:weizhenyu@swufe.edu.cn基金资助:西南财经大学中央高校基本科 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 敬畏的心理模型及其认知神经机制
    赵小红1,2,童薇3,陈桃林1,2(),吴冬梅4,张蕾1,2,陈正举1,方晓义3,龚启勇1,2,唐小蓉21四川大学华西医院放射科华西磁共振研究中心,成都6100412四川大学公共管理学院社会学与心理学系,成都6100653北京师范大学心理学部,北京1008754成都市第四人民医院,成都610036收 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 个体采纳与群体采纳:决策过程中的两类建议采纳行为
    任小云1,段锦云2,冯成志1()1苏州大学教育学院,苏州2151232华东师范大学心理与认知科学学院,上海200062收稿日期:2020-06-30出版日期:2021-03-15发布日期:2021-01-26通讯作者:冯成志E-mail:fengchengzhi@suda.edu.cn基金资助:国家 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 老年人心理韧性与幸福感的关系:一项元分析
    叶静,张戌凡()南京师范大学金陵女子学院,南京210097收稿日期:2020-07-23出版日期:2021-02-15发布日期:2020-12-29通讯作者:张戌凡E-mail:xufanzhang@163.com基金资助:国家自然科学青年基金项目“工会实践对员工工作幸福感的影响:基于工具-情感的双 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 基于网络理论的物质成瘾新视角
    刘宇1,胡传鹏2(),樊富珉1(),孙沛1,徐杰3,蔡玉清1,刘雪莉41清华大学心理学系,北京1000843北京高新医院,北京1000014北京社康社会工作服务中心,北京100001收稿日期:2019-12-17出版日期:2021-02-15发布日期:2020-12-29通讯作者:胡传鹏,樊富珉E- ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 催产素调控心理韧性:基于对海马的作用机制
    薛冰,王雪娇,马宁,高军()认知与人格教育部重点实验室,重庆400715收稿日期:2020-04-27出版日期:2021-02-15发布日期:2020-12-29通讯作者:高军E-mail:gaojunscience@126.com基金资助:国家自然科学基金(32071059);重庆市自然科学基金( ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 计算模型视角下信任形成的心理和神经机制——基于信任博弈中投资者的角度
    高青林,周媛()中国科学院心理研究所行为科学重点实验室,北京100101中国科学院大学心理学系,北京100049收稿日期:2020-04-18出版日期:2021-01-15发布日期:2020-11-23通讯作者:周媛E-mail:zhouyuan@psych.ac.cn基金资助:*中国科学院心理研究 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 《心理科学进展》2020年度审稿专家名录
    出版日期:2021-01-15发布日期:2021-01-08Online:2021-01-15Published:2021-01-08摘要/Abstract参考文献相关文章0Norelatedarticlesfound!PDF全文下载地址:http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlk ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01