删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

肢体形状和肢体动作表征的早期发展

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

杜碧煊, 张明明, 张珂烨, 任杰, 何蔚祺()
辽宁师范大学脑与认知神经科学研究中心, 大连 116029
收稿日期:2019-01-21出版日期:2019-10-15发布日期:2019-08-19
通讯作者:何蔚祺E-mail:weiqi79920686@sina.com

基金资助:* 国家自然科学基金青年项目资助(81601166)

Early development of the body shape and body movement in infancy

DU Bixuan, ZHANG Mingming, ZHANG Keye, REN Jie, HE Weiqi()
Research Center of Brain and Cognitive Neuroscience, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
Received:2019-01-21Online:2019-10-15Published:2019-08-19
Contact:HE Weiqi E-mail:weiqi79920686@sina.com






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 肢体是社会性信息的重要载体。研究肢体表征的早期发展对于揭示其肢体表征加工的内在机制, 进一步理解婴儿的社会性发展具有重要意义。从肢体形状和肢体动作两个方面来看, 已有研究主要考察了婴儿在处理肢体结构信息和运动信息时的表征方式, 并提出了婴儿肢体表征发展机制的两种理论。未来应进一步明确婴儿肢体表征的具体发展过程及关键期, 并通过脑成像技术探究生物运动偏好对于肢体表征的作用机制。


[1] 蒋毅, 王莉 . ( 2011). 生物运动加工特异性:整体结构和局部运动的作用. 心理科学进展, 19( 3), 301-311.
[2] 张珂烨, 张明明, 刘田田, 罗文波, 何蔚祺 . ( 2019). 肢体识别的倒置效应. 心理科学进展, 27( 1), 27-36.
[3] Alexander G. M., Hawkins L. B., Wilcox T., & Hirshkowitz A . ( 2016). Infants prefer female body phenotypes; Infant girls prefer they have an hourglass shape. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 804.
[4] Aviezer H., Trope Y., & Todorov A . ( 2012). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science, 338( 6111), 1225.
[5] Axelsson E. L., Moore D. G., Murphy E. M., Goodwin J. E., & Clifford B. R . ( 2018). The role of bodies in infants' categorical representations of humans and non-human animals. Infant and Child Development, 27(6).
[6] Bachmann J., Munzert J., & Krüger B . ( 2018). Neural underpinnings of the perception of emotional states derived from biological human motion: A review of neuroimaging research. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1763.
[7] Bardi L., Regolin L., & Simion F . ( 2011). Biological motion preference in humans at birth: Role of dynamic and configural properties. Developmental Science, 14( 2), 353-359.
[8] Bertenthal B. I., Proffitt D. R., & Cutting J. E . ( 1984). Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37( 2), 213-230.
[9] Bertenthal B. I., Proffitt D. R., & Kramer S. J . ( 1987). Perception of biomechanical motions by infants: implementation of various processing constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13( 4), 577-585.
[10] Bhatt R. S., Hock A., White H., Jubran R., & Galati A . ( 2016). The development of body structure knowledge in infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 10( 1), 45-52.
[11] Bidet-Ildei C., Kitromilides E., Orliaguet J.-P., Pavlova M., & Gentaz E . ( 2014). Preference for point-light human biological motion in newborns: Contribution of translational displacement. Developmental Psychology, 50( 1), 113-120.
[12] Blake, R. ( 1993). Cats perceive biological motion. Psychological Science, 4( 1), 54-57
[13] Booth A. E., Pinto J., & Bertenthal B. I . ( 2002). Perception of the symmetrical patterning of human gait by infants. Developmental Psychology, 38( 4), 554-563.
[14] Bottari D., Troje N. F., Ley P., Hense M., Kekunnaya R., & Röder B . ( 2015). The neural development of the biological motion processing system does not rely on early visual input. Cortex, 71, 359-367.
[15] Chang D. H. F., Ban H., Ikegaya Y., Fujita I., & Troje N. F . ( 2018). Cortical and subcortical responses to biological motion. Neuroimage, 174, 87-96.
[16] Chang D. H. F., & Troje, N. F. ( 2009). Characterizing global and local mechanisms in biological motion perception. Journal of Vision, 9( 5), 8 1-10.
[17] Christie T., & Slaughter V. ( 2010). Movement contributes to infants' recognition of the human form. Cognition, 114( 3), 329-337.
[18] de Gelder B., de Borst A. W., & Watson R . ( 2015). The perception of emotion in body expressions. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6( 2), 149-158.
[19] Deen B., Richardson H., Dilks D. D., Takahashi A., Keil B., Wald L. L., .. Saxe R . ( 2017). Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants. Nature Communications, 8, 13995.
[20] di Giorgio E., Loveland J. L., Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., Versace E., & Vallortigara G . ( 2017). Filial responses as predisposed and learned preferences: Early attachment in chicks and babies. Behavioural Brain Research, 325, 90-104.
[21] Fox R., & McDaniel C. ( 1982). The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science, 218( 4571), 486-487.
[22] Gliga T., & Dehaene-Lambertz G. ( 2005). Structural encoding of body and face in human infants and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17( 8), 1328-1340.
[23] Greenough W. T., Black J. E., & Wallace C. S . ( 1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development, 58( 3), 539-559.
[24] Harris A., Vyas D. B., & Reed C. L . ( 2016). Holistic processing for bodies and body parts: New evidence from stereoscopic depth manipulations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23( 5), 1513-1519.
[25] Heron-Delaney M., Wirth S., & Pascalis O . ( 2011). Infants' knowledge of their own species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 366( 1571), 1753-1763.
[26] Heron M., & Slaughter V. ( 2010). Infants' responses to real humans and representations of humans. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34( 1), 34-45.
[27] Hock A., Kangas A., Zieber N., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2015). The development of sex category representation in infancy: Matching of faces and bodies. Developmental Psychology, 51( 3), 346-352.
[28] Hock A., White H., Jubran R., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2016). The whole picture: Holistic body posture recognition in infancy. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23( 2), 426-431.
[29] Israelashvili J., Hassin R. R., & Aviezer H . ( 2018). When emotions run high: A critical role for context in the unfolding of dynamic, real-life facial affect. Emotion, in press.
[30] Johansson, G. ( 1973). Visual-perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 14( 2), 201-211.
[31] Kirby L. A., Moraczewski D., Warnell K., Velnoskey K., & Redcay E . ( 2018). Social network size relates to developmental neural sensitivity to biological motion. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 169-177.
[32] Longhi E., Senna I., Bolognini N., Bulf H., Tagliabue P., Cassia V. M., & Turati C . ( 2015). Discrimination of biomechanically possible and impossible hand movements at birth. Child Development, 86( 2), 632-641.
[33] Lorenzi E., Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., & Vallortigara G . ( 2017). Dynamic features of animate motion activate septal and preoptic areas in visually naïve chicks (gallus gallus). Neuroscience, 354, 54-68.
[34] Marinović V., Hoehl S., & Pauen S . ( 2014). Neural correlates of human-animal distinction: An ERP-study on early categorical differentiation with 4- and 7-month-old infants and adults. Neuropsychologia, 60, 60-76.
[35] Marshall P. J., & Meltzoff, A. N. ( 2015). Body maps in the infant brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19( 9), 499-505.
[36] Marshall P. J., & Shipley, T. F. ( 2009). Event-related potentials to point-light displays of human actions in five-month-old infants. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34( 3), 368-377.
[37] Maurer D., Le Grand R., & Mondloch C. J . ( 2002). The many faces of configural processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6( 6), 255-260.
[38] Moore D. G., Goodwin J. E., George R., Axelsson E. L., & Braddick, F. M. B . ( 2007). Infants perceive human point-light displays as solid forms. Cognition, 104( 2), 377-396.
[39] Morita T., Slaughter V., Katayama N., Kitazaki M., Kakigi R., & Itakura S . ( 2012). Infant and adult perceptions of possible and impossible body movements: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113( 3), 401-414.
[40] Morton J., & Johnson, M. H. ( 1991). Conspec and conlern: A 2-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98( 2), 164-181.
[41] Peelen M. V., & Downing, P. E. ( 2007). The neural basis of visual body perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8( 8), 636-648.
[42] Peelen M. V., Glaser B., Vuilleumier P., & Eliez S . ( 2009). Differential development of selectivity for faces and bodies in the fusiform gyrus. Developmental Science, 12( 6), F16-F25.
[43] Peykarjou S., Wissner J., & Pauen S . ( 2015). Categorical ERP repetition effects for human and furniture items in 7-month-old infants. Infant and Child Development, 26( 5), 1-12.
[44] Reed C. L., Stone V. E., Bozova S., & Tanaka J . ( 2003). The body-inversion effect. Psychological Science, 14( 4), 302-308.
[45] Reed C. L., Stone V. E., Grubb J. D., & McGoldrick J. E . ( 2006). Turning configural processing upside down: Part and whole body postures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32( 1), 73-87.
[46] Reid V. M., Hoehl S., Landt J., & Striano T . ( 2008). Human infants dissociate structural and dynamic information in biological motion: Evidence from neural systems. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3( 2), 161-167.
[47] Reid V. M., Hoehl S., & Striano T . ( 2006). The perception of biological motion by infants: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 395( 3), 211-214
[48] Reid V. M., Kaduk K., & Lunn J . ( 2019). Links between action perception and ac.tion production in 10-week-old infants. Neuropsychologia, 126, 69-74.
[49] Seitz, K. ( 2002). Parts and wholes in person recognition: Developmental trends. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82( 4), 367-381.
[50] Shi Y., Ma X., Ma Z., Wang J., Yao N., Gu Q., .. Gao Z . ( 2018). Using a kinect sensor to acquire biological motion: Toolbox and evaluation. Behavior Research Methods, 50( 2), 518-529
[51] Sifre R., Olson L., Gillespie S., Klin A., Jones W., & Shultz S . ( 2018). A longitudinal investigation of preferential attention to biological motion in 2- to 24-month-old infants. Scientific Reports, 8( 1), 2527.
[52] Simion F., Regolin L., & Bulf H . ( 2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105( 2), 809-813.
[53] Slaughter V., & Heron M. ( 2004). Origins and early development of human body knowledge. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69( 2), 1-102.
[54] Slaughter V., Heron M., & Sim S . ( 2002). Development of preferences for the human body shape in infancy. Cognition, 85( 3), B71-B81.
[55] Stekelenburg J. J., & de Gelder , B. ( 2004). The neural correlates of perceiving human bodies: An ERP study on the body-inversion effect. Neuroreport, 15( 5), 777-780.
[56] Taylor J. C., Wiggett A. J., & Downing P. E . ( 2007). Functional MRI analysis of body and body part representations in the extrastriate and fusiform body areas. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98( 3), 1626-1633.
[57] Wenzler S., Levine S., van Dick R., Oertel-Knöchel V., & Aviezer H . ( 2016). Beyond pleasure and pain: Facial expression ambiguity in adults and children during intense situations. Emotion, 16( 6), 807-814.
[58] White H., Hock A., Jubran R., Heck A., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2018). Visual scanning of male and female bodies in infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 79-95.
[59] White H., Jubran R., Heck A., Chroust A., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2019). Sex-specific scanning in infancy: Developmental changes in the use of face/head and body information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 182, 126-143.
[60] Zieber N., Bhatt R. S., Hayden A., Kangas A., Collins R., & Bada H . ( 2010). Body representation in the first year of life. Infancy, 15( 5), 534-544.
[61] Zieber N., Kangas A., Hock A., & Bhatt R. S . ( 2015). Body structure perception in infancy. Infancy, 20( 1), 1-17.




[1]程羽慧, 袁祥勇, 蒋毅. 社会互动加工的认知特性及脑机制——第三人称的视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(3): 472-480.
[2]魏华, 汪涛, 毛磊, 冯文婷, 熊莎莎. 叠音品牌名称对消费者知觉和态度的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(7): 1071-1082.
[3]毕小彬, 范晓壮, 米文丽, 贺荟中. 高风险婴儿前瞻性纵向研究与孤独症谱系障碍早期识别[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(3): 443-455.
[4]刘慧晨, 陈坚. 婴儿的痛觉[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(10): 1723-1732.
[5]程刚, 贾云丞, 丁芳媛, 张大均, 陈加, 龙女. 表情对婴儿面孔图式效应的调节及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(5): 761-772.
[6]张珂烨, 张明明, 刘田田, 罗文波, 何蔚祺. 肢体识别的倒置效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(1): 27-36.
[7]杨伟星;张明亮;李红霞;杨雅琳;司继伟. 人类基本数学能力的进化证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(5): 810-824.
[8]黄琪钰;刘秀丽. 婴儿是否拥有错误信念理解能力?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(3): 431-442.
[9]钱浩悦;朱敏;高湘萍. 老年人面孔构形加工特点[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(2): 230-236.
[10]许少月, 王争艳. 婴儿期外显问题行为的发生与表现及成因分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(11): 1910-1921.
[11]伍珍, 郭睿. 婴儿指示性手势与其语言学习的关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(10): 1705-1712.
[12]周玉;张丹丹. 婴儿情绪与社会认知相关的听觉加工[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(1): 67-75.
[13]李琎; 范伟; 张文洁; 钟毅平. 旋转自我面孔识别及其认知机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 50-.
[14]窦东徽;刘肖岑;张玉洁. 娃娃脸效应:对婴儿面孔的偏好及过度泛化[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(5): 760-771.
[15]皮忠玲;莫书亮. 婴儿心理理论的发展:表现和机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(8): 1408-1421.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4802
相关话题/心理 科学 运动 信息 生物

  • 领限时大额优惠券,享本站正版考研考试资料!
    大额优惠券
    优惠券领取后72小时内有效,10万种最新考研考试考证类电子打印资料任你选。涵盖全国500余所院校考研专业课、200多种职业资格考试、1100多种经典教材,产品类型包含电子书、题库、全套资料以及视频,无论您是考研复习、考证刷题,还是考前冲刺等,不同类型的产品可满足您学习上的不同需求。 ...
    本站小编 Free壹佰分学习网 2022-09-19
  • 反社会人格障碍的神经生物学基础及其司法启示
    刘宇平,赵辉,李姗珊,张卓,杨波()中国政法大学社会学院,北京100088收稿日期:2018-12-12出版日期:2019-10-15发布日期:2019-08-19通讯作者:杨波E-mail:zsdybo@sina.com基金资助:*国家重点研发计划项目(2018YFC0831002);国家社会科学 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 为什么信息超载损害决策?基于有限认知资源的解释
    车敬上1,孙海龙2,肖晨洁1,李爱梅1()1暨南大学管理学院,广州5106322广东外语外贸大学商学院,广州510420收稿日期:2018-12-20出版日期:2019-10-15发布日期:2019-08-19通讯作者:李爱梅E-mail:tliaim@jnu.edu.cn基金资助:*国家自然科学基 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 社会排斥的心理行为特征及其脑机制
    彭苏浩,陶丹,冷玥(),邓慧华东南大学儿童发展与教育研究所,南京210096收稿日期:2019-01-14出版日期:2019-09-15发布日期:2019-07-24通讯作者:冷玥E-mail:lengyue@seu.edu.cn基金资助:国家自然科学基金项目(31500881);江苏省自然科学基金 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 1993至2016年医学生心理健康变迁的横断历史研究
    辛素飞1(),姜文源1,辛自强21鲁东大学教育科学学院,烟台2640112中央财经大学社会与心理学院,北京100081收稿日期:2018-09-14出版日期:2019-07-15发布日期:2019-05-22通讯作者:辛素飞E-mail:xinsufei2005@163.com基金资助:*国家社会科 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 面孔表情和声音情绪信息整合加工的脑机制
    李萍,张明明,李帅霞,张火垠,罗文波()辽宁师范大学脑与认知神经科学研究中心,大连116029收稿日期:2018-05-17出版日期:2019-07-15发布日期:2019-05-22通讯作者:罗文波E-mail:luowb@lnnu.edu.cn基金资助:*国家自然科学基金项目资助(3187110 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 素食的心理过程及影响因素
    刘潇肖1,田启瑞2(),曾雅丽11厦门大学管理学院,厦门3610052山东师范大学心理学院,济南250358收稿日期:2018-08-12出版日期:2019-07-15发布日期:2019-05-22通讯作者:田启瑞E-mail:tianqirui@sdnu.edu.cn基金资助:*国家自然科学基金青 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 群体信息加工视角下团队建言有效性及作用机制:建言数量与质量的影响
    李方君(),王舒曼,李爱梅,李斌暨南大学管理学院,广州510632收稿日期:2018-06-21出版日期:2019-06-15发布日期:2019-04-22通讯作者:李方君E-mail:lifangjun@jnu.edu.cn基金资助:国家自然科学基金项目(71602076);国家自然科学基金项目( ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 和谐医患关系的心理机制及其促进技术
    孙连荣1,王沛2()1上海师范大学天华学院,上海2018152华东师范大学教育学部特殊教育系,上海200062收稿日期:2018-03-07出版日期:2019-06-15发布日期:2019-04-22通讯作者:王沛E-mail:wangpei1970@163.com基金资助:国家社会科学重大招标项目 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 风险决策的概率权重偏差:心理机制与优化策略
    孙庆洲,邬青渊,张静,江程铭,赵雷,胡凤培()浙江工业大学管理学院,杭州310023收稿日期:2018-08-23出版日期:2019-05-15发布日期:2019-03-20通讯作者:胡凤培E-mail:fengpei@zjut.edu.cn基金资助:*教育部人文社科青年基金项目(18YJC6301 ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01
  • 不道德传染的心理机制及其影响
    王悦1,龚园超1,2,3,李莹1()1郑州大学教育学院,郑州4500012中国科学院心理研究所行为科学重点实验室,北京1001013中国科学院大学心理学系,北京100049收稿日期:2018-06-05出版日期:2019-04-15发布日期:2019-02-22通讯作者:李莹E-mail:liyin ...
    本站小编 Free考研考试 2022-01-01