浙江工业大学管理学院, 杭州 310023
收稿日期:
2018-08-23出版日期:
2019-05-15发布日期:
2019-03-20通讯作者:
胡凤培E-mail:fengpei@zjut.edu.cn基金资助:
* 教育部人文社科青年基金项目(18YJC630155);国家自然科学基金青年项目(No.71801193);国家社科基金(No.14BSH070)Probability weighting bias in risky decision making: Psychological mechanism and optimizing strategies
SUN Qingzhou, WU Qingyuan, ZHANG Jing, JIANG Chengming, ZHAO Lei, HU Fengpei()School of Management, Zhejiang University of Technology, Hangzhou 310023, China
Received:
2018-08-23Online:
2019-05-15Published:
2019-03-20Contact:
HU Fengpei E-mail:fengpei@zjut.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 概率权重偏差指人对事件发生的主观概率估计与客观概率的差异。它影响投资、投保、医患沟通等方面。“重结果轻概率”的非补偿性策略和参照点诱发的情绪波动会引发概率权重偏差; 改变“概率”的描述形式、“结果”的情绪体验、“损益”的参照点、风险的心理距离等可调整权重偏差、优化决策。未来需深究权重偏差的适用情境、机制关联及偏差辨别等问题。
图/表 3
图1概率权重偏差的辨别力参数(左)和吸引力参数(右)
图1概率权重偏差的辨别力参数(左)和吸引力参数(右)
图2概率权重偏差的“注意分配”解释机制示意图
图2概率权重偏差的“注意分配”解释机制示意图
图3概率权重偏差的“参照点-情绪体验”解释机制示意图
图3概率权重偏差的“参照点-情绪体验”解释机制示意图
参考文献 50
[1] | 吉仁泽(德) 泽尔腾(德) , ( 2016). 有限理性: 适应性工具箱 (刘永芳译). 清华大学出版社. 北京. |
[2] | 李纾 . ( 2016). 决策心理: 齐当别之道. 华东师范大学出版社. 上海. |
[3] | 梁哲, 李纾, 许洁虹 . ( 2007). 预期理论权重函数π的由来, 质疑及Tversky的阐释. 经济数学, 24( 4), 331-340. |
[4] | 孙庆洲 . ( 2017). 社会距离对概率估计偏差的影响:情绪占优抑或认知占优?(博士学位论文). 华东师范大学. 上海. |
[5] | 汪祚军, 李纾 . ( 2012). 对整合模型和占优启发式模型的检验: 基于信息加工过程的眼动研究证据. 心理学报, 44( 2), 179-198. |
[6] | 新浪科技. ( 2018). 滴滴:2017年订单量74.3亿平均每人用滴滴打过5次车. 2018年1月8摘自 |
[7] | 中国消费者协会. ( 2019). 2018年十大消费维权舆情热点. 2019年1月10摘自 |
[8] | 中国新闻网. ( 2018). 女孩滴滴顺风车遇害案被告人被提起公诉案件回顾. 2018年11月17摘自 |
[9] | Athey S., .( 2017). Beyond prediction: Using big data for policy problems. Science, 355( 6324), 483-485. doi: 10.1126/science.aal4321URL |
[10] | Brandstätter E., Gigerenzer G., & Hertwig R . ( 2006). The priority heuristic: Making choices without trade-offs. Psychological Review, 113( 2), 409-432. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.409URL |
[11] | Brandstätter E., Kühberger A., & Schneider F . ( 2002). A cognitive-emotional account of the shape of the probability weighting function. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15( 2), 79-100. doi: 10.1002/bdm.v15:2URL |
[12] | Diederich A., & Oswald P. , ( 2016). Multi-stage sequential sampling models with finite or infinite time horizon and variable boundaries. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 74, 128-145. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2016.02.010URL |
[13] | Evans J. S. B. T., Handley S. J., Perham N., Over D. E., & Thompson V. A . ( 2000). Frequency versus probability formats in statistical word problems. Cognition, 77( 3), 197-213. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00098-6URL |
[14] | Fagerlin A., Zikmund-Fisher B. J., & Ubel P. A . ( 2011). Helping patients decide: Ten steps to better risk communication. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 103( 19), 1436-1443. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djr318URL |
[15] | Faro D. & Rottenstreich Y. , ( 2006). Affect, empathy, and regressive mispredictions of others’ preferences under risk. Management Science, 52( 4), 529-541. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.1050.0490URL |
[16] | Fisher G. , ( 2017). An attentional drift diffusion model over binary-attribute choice. Cognition, 168, 34-45. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.007URL |
[17] | Fox C. R., & Poldrack R. A . ( 2009). Prospect theory and the brain. Neuroeconomics: Decision Making and the Brain, 145-174. |
[18] | Gonzalez R. & Wu G. , ( 1999). On the shape of the probability weighting function. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 129-166. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0710URL |
[19] | Hansen J. V., Jacobsen R. H., & Lau M. I . ( 2016). Willingness to pay for insurance in Denmark. Journal of Risk and Insurance, 83( 1), 49-76. doi: 10.1111/jori.v83.1URL |
[20] | Hey J. D., Morone A., & Schmidt U . ( 2009). Noise and bias in eliciting preferences. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 39( 3), 213-235. doi: 10.1007/s11166-009-9081-1URL |
[21] | Kahneman D. & Tversky A. , ( 1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47( 2), 267-291. |
[22] | Kliger D. & Levy O. , ( 2008). Mood impacts on probability weighting functions: “Large-gamble” evidence. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 37( 4), 1397-1411. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2007.08.010URL |
[23] | Lermer E., Streicher B., Sachs R., Raue M., & Frey D . ( 2015). Thinking concrete increases the perceived likelihood of risks: The effect of construal level on risk estimation. Risk Analysis, 36( 3), 623-637. |
[24] | Lichtenstein S., & Slovic P. , ( 1971). Reversals of preference between bids and choices in gambling decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 8 9(1), 46-55. |
[25] | Li L. B., He S. H., Li S., Xu J. H., & Rao L. L . ( 2009). A closer look at the Russian roulette problem: A re-examination of the nonlinearity of the prospect theory’s decision weight π. International Journal of Approximate Reasoning, 50( 3), 515-520. doi: 10.1016/j.ijar.2008.10.004URL |
[26] | Li S., . ( 1995). Is there a decision weight π? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 27( 3), 263-291. |
[27] | Litchfield J., & Piza C. , ( 2017). Estimating the willingness to pay for tenure security in Brazilian Favelas (working paper series). Department of Economic, University of Sussex. |
[28] | McGraw A. P., Todorov A., & Kunreuther H . ( 2011). A policy maker’s dilemma: Preventing terrorism or preventing blame. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115( 1), 25-34. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.01.004URL |
[29] | Mullett T. L., & Stewart N. , ( 2016). Implications of visual attention phenomena for models of preferential choice. Decision, 3( 4), 231-253. doi: 10.1037/dec0000049URL |
[30] | Oudhoff J. P., & Timmermans D. R . ( 2015). The effect of different graphical and numerical likelihood formats on perception of likelihood and choice. Medical Decision Making, 35( 4), 487-500. doi: 10.1177/0272989X15576487URL |
[31] | Pachur T., Hertwig R., & Wolkewitz R . ( 2014). The affect gap in risky choice: Affect-rich outcomes attenuate attention to probability information. Decision, 1( 1), 64-78. doi: 10.1037/dec0000006URL |
[32] | Pachur T., Schulte-Mecklenbeck M., Murphy R. O., & Hertwig R . ( 2018). Prospect theory reflects selective allocation of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147( 2), 147-169. doi: 10.1037/xge0000406URL |
[33] | Pachur T., Suter R. S., & Hertwig R . ( 2017). How the twain can meet: Prospect theory and models of heuristics in risky choice. Cognitive Psychology, 93, 44-73. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.01.001URL |
[34] | Pahlke J., Strasser S., & Vieider F. M . ( 2015). Responsibility effects in decision making under risk. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 51( 2), 125-146. doi: 10.1007/s11166-015-9223-6URL |
[35] | Prelec D., Seung H. S., & McCoy J . ( 2017). A solution to the single-question crowd wisdom problem. Nature, 541, 532-535. doi: 10.1038/nature21054URL |
[36] | Rottenstreich Y., & Hsee C. K . ( 2001). Money, kisses, and electric shocks: On the affective psychology of risk. Psychological Science, 12( 3), 185-190. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00334URL |
[37] | Stone E. R., Yates J. F., & Parker A. M . ( 1997). Effects of numerical and graphical displays on professed risk-taking behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3( 4), 243-256. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.3.4.243URL |
[38] | Sun Q. Z., Liu Y. F., Zhang H. R., & Lu J. Y . ( 2017). Increased social distance makes people more risk-neutral. The Journal of Social Psychology, 157( 4), 502-512. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2016.1242471URL |
[39] | Sun Q. Z., Zhang H. R., Sai L. Y., & Hu F. P . ( 2018). Self-distancing reduces probability-weighting biases. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 611. |
[40] | Sun Q. Z., Zhang H. R., Zhang J., & Zhang X. N . ( 2018). Why can’t we accurately predict others’ decisions? Prediction discrepancy in risky decision-making. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2190. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02190URL |
[41] | Suter R. S., Pachur T., & Hertwig R . ( 2016). How affect shapes risky choice: Distorted probability weighting versus probability neglect. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29( 4), 437-449. |
[42] | Suter R. S., Pachur T., Hertwig R., Endestad T., & Biele G . ( 2015). The neural basis of risky choice with affective outcomes. PloS One, 10, e0122475. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122475URL |
[43] | Trautmann S. T., & van de Kuilen G. , ( 2012). Prospect theory or construal level theory? Diminishing sensitivity vs. psychological distance in risky decisions. Acta Psychologica, 139( 1), 254-260. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.08.006URL |
[44] | Trope Y., & Liberman N. , ( 2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117( 2), 440-463. doi: 10.1037/a0018963URL |
[45] | Tversky A., & Kahneman D. ,( 1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5( 4), 297-323. doi: 10.1007/BF00122574URL |
[46] | Tyszka T. & Sawicki P. , ( 2011). Affective and cognitive factors influencing sensitivity to probabilistic information. Risk Analysis, 31( 11), 1832-1845. doi: 10.1111/risk.2011.31.issue-11URL |
[47] | Venkatraman V., Payne J. W., & Huettel S. A . ( 2014). An overall probability of winning heuristic for complex risky decisions: Choice and eye fixation evidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 125( 2), 73-87. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.06.003URL |
[48] | Wang Z. J., Kuang Y., Tang H. Y., Gao C., Chen A., & Chan K. Q . ( 2018). Are decisions made by group representatives more risk averse? The effect of sense of responsibility. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31( 3), 311-323. doi: 10.1002/bdm.v31.3URL |
[49] | Williams L. E., Stein R., & Galguera L . ( 2014). The distinct affective consequences of psychological distance and construal level. Journal of Consumer Research, 40( 6), 1123-1138. doi: 10.1086/674212URL |
[50] | Zipkin D. A., Umscheid C. A., Keating N. L., Allen E., Aung K., Beyth R., .. Feldstein D. A . ( 2014). Evidence-based risk communication: A systematic review. Annals of Internal Medicine, 161( 4), 270-280. doi: 10.7326/M14-0295URL |
相关文章 9
[1] | 周璨, 周临舒, 蒋存梅. 音乐愉悦体验的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(1): 123-130. |
[2] | 宋云嫱;徐瑞珩;邢采. 风险敏感理论:需要驱动风险决策[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(3): 486-499. |
[3] | 熊冠星;李爱梅;王晓田. 基于三参照点理论的薪酬差距与离职决策的分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(9): 1363-1371. |
[4] | 谢晓非;陆静怡. 风险决策中的双参照点效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(4): 571-579. |
[5] | 敖玲敏;吕厚超;庞雪. “悲喜交加”的概念、测量及相关研究述评[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(9): 1643-1650. |
[6] | 王晓田;王鹏. 决策的三参照点理论:从原理到应用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(8): 1331-1346. |
[7] | 陈景秋;唐宁玉;王方华;C. K. Hsee. 从幸福学角度对和谐消费的阐释[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(7): 1081-1086. |
[8] | 杨晓红; 张志杰 . 时间隐喻中的空间参照框架 [J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(1): 1-9. |
[9] | 何贵兵,于永菊. 决策过程中参照点效应研究述评[J]. 心理科学进展, 2006, 14(3): 408-412. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4685