
广州大学教育学院心理系, 脑与认知科学中心, 广州 510006
收稿日期:
2018-06-20出版日期:
2018-11-15发布日期:
2018-09-26通讯作者:
傅世敏E-mail:fusm@gzhu.edu.cn基金资助:
* 国家自然科学基金资助项目(31371142)Research debate: Does spatial attention modulate C1 component?
FU Shimin(
Department of Psychology and Center for Brain and Cognitive Sciences, School of Education, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou 510006, China
Received:
2018-06-20Online:
2018-11-15Published:
2018-09-26Contact:
FU Shimin E-mail:fusm@gzhu.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 视觉注意的事件相关电位研究中, 对源于V1的C1成分是否受空间注意调制存在争议。多数派观点认为C1不受空间注意直接调制, 但存在对V1的延迟反馈调节; 少数派观点则认为在一定的实验条件下, 空间注意可以在视觉信息加工的早期前馈加工阶段直接调制C1成分。近期双方就此问题展开了讨论。本综述中, 我们首先概述了双方基本观点和实验证据。其次, 简要概括了C1注意效应的多个影响因素。再次, 就近期关于C1注意效应的可重复性、注意负载与知觉负载对诱发C1注意效应的作用、极性翻转与源于V1的关系这三方面的争论进行了述评。最后, 针对该研究争论我们提出了两个原创性观点:其一, 需以开放而又谨慎的态度对待C1注意效应; 其二, 有些方法和技巧可能有助于观察到潜在的C1注意效应。综上, 多数派观点已得到大量实验证据支持, 而少数派观点尚需提供更加决定性的实验证据; 该项争论还将继续。
参考文献 55
[1] | 陈建, 袁杰, 汪海玲, 王妍, 傅世敏 . ( 2013). C1 调制效应的理论评述及影响因素. 心理科学进展, 21( 3), 407-417 doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00407URL |
[2] | Ales, J. M., Yates, J. L., & Norcia, A. M. ( 2010). V1 is not uniquely identified by polarity reversals of responses to upper and lower visual field stimuli. Neuroimage, 52( 4), 1401-1409. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.016URL |
[3] | Ales, J. M., Yates, J. L., & Norcia, A. M. ( 2013). On determining the intercranial sources of visual evoked potentials from scalp topography: A reply to Kelly et al. (this issue). Neuroimage, 64, 703-711. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.009URL |
[4] | Bahrami, B., Carmel, D., Walsh, V., Rees, G., & Lavie, N. ( 2008). Spatial attention can modulate unconscious orientation processing. Perception, 37( 10), 1520-1528. doi: 10.1068/p5999URLpmid: 19065856 |
[5] | Baumgartner, H. M., Graulty, C. J., Hillyard, S. A., & Pitts, M. A. ( 2018). Does spatial attention modulate the C1 component? The jury continues to deliberate. Cognitive Neuroscience, 9( 1-2), 34-37. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2017.1386169URLpmid: 28956499 |
[6] | Bayer, M., Rossi, V., Vanlessen, N., Grass, A., Schacht, A., & Pourtois, G. ( 2017). Independent effects of motivation and spatial attention in the human visual cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12( 1), 146-156. |
[7] | Clark, V.P., & Hillyard, S.A . ( 1996). Spatial selective attention affects early extrastriate but not striate components of the visual evoked potential. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8( 5), 387-402. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.5.387URL |
[8] | Dassanayake, T. L., Michie, P. T., & Fulham, R. ( 2016). Effect of temporal predictability on exogenous attentional modulation of feedforward processing in the striate cortex. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 105, 9-16. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.04.007URL |
[9] | Desimone, R., & Duncan, J.( 1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193-222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205URL |
[10] | Di Russo, F., Martinez, A., & Hillyard, S. A. ( 2003). Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo- spatial attention. Cerebral Cortex, 13( 5), 486-499. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.5.486URL |
[11] | Ding, Y. L., Martinez, A., Qu, Z., & Hillyard, S. A. ( 2014). Earliest stages of visual cortical processing are not modified by attentional load. Human Brain Mapping, 35( 7), 3008-3024. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22381URL |
[12] | Fu, S.M. ( 2018a). Open and cautious towards the “minority view”. Cognitive Neuroscience, 9( 1-2), 28-30. |
[13] | Fu, S.M. ( 2018b). ‘Tricks’ for revealing potential attentional modulations on the C1 component. Cognitive Neuroscience, 9( 1-2), 63-64. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2017.1384376URLpmid: 28944720 |
[14] | Fu, S. M., Caggiano, D. M., Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. ( 2005b). Event-related potentials reveal dissociable mechanisms for orienting and focusing visuospatial attention. Cognitive Brain Research, 23( 2-3), 341-353. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.11.014URL |
[15] | Fu, S. M.,Fan, S. L.,Chen, L., & Zhuo, Y. ( 2001). The attentional effects of peripheral cueing as revealed by two event-related potential studies. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112( 1), 172-185. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00500-9URLpmid: 11137676 |
[16] | Fu, S. M., Fedota, J., Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. ( 2010a). Early interaction between perceptual load and involuntary attention: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 468( 1), 68-71. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.10.065URL |
[17] | Fu, S. M., Fedota, J. R., Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. ( 2010b). Dissociation of visual C1 and P1 components as a function of attentional load: an event-related potential study. Biological Psychology, 85( 1), 171-178. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.06.008URL |
[18] | Fu, S. M., Fedota, J. R., Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. ( 2012). Attentional load is not a critical factor for eliciting C1 attentional effect - A reply to Rauss, Pourtois, Vuillenumier, and Schwartz. Biological Psychology, 91( 2), 321-324. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.012URL |
[19] | Fu, S.M.,Greenwood, P.M., & Parasuraman, R.( 2005a). Brain mechanisms of involuntary visuospatial attention: An event-related potential study. Human Brain Mapping, 25( 4), 378-390. doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1097-0193URL |
[20] | Fu, S. M., Huang, Y. X., Luo, Y. J., Wang, Y., Fedota, J., Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. ( 2009). Perceptual load interacts with involuntary attention at early processing stages: Event-related potential studies. Neuroimage, 48( 1), 191-199. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.028URL |
[21] | Fu, S. M., Zinni, M., Squire, P. N., Kumar, R., Caggiano, D. M., & Parasuraman, R. ( 2008). When and where perceptual load interacts with voluntary visuospatial attention: An event-related potential and dipole modeling study. Neuroimage, 39( 3), 1345-1355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.068URL |
[22] | Heinze, H. J., Mangun, G. R., Burchert, W., Hinrichs, H., Scholz, M., Münte, T. F., ... Hillyard, S. A. ( 1994). Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature, 372, 543-546. doi: 10.1038/372543a0URL |
[23] | Herde, L., Rossi, V., Pourtois, G., & Rauss, K. ( 2018). Early retinotopic responses to violations of emotion-location associations may depend on conscious awareness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 9( 1-2), 38-55. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2017.1338250URL |
[24] | Hillyard, S. A., Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. ( 1998). Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: Electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 353( 1373), 1257-1270. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0281URL |
[25] | Hopfinger, J.B., & Mangun, G.R . ( 1998). Reflexive attention modulates processing of visual stimuli in human extrastriate cortex. Psychological Science, 9( 6), 441-447. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00083URLpmid: 4552358 |
[26] | Jeffreys, D.A., & Axford, J.G . ( 1972a). Source locations of pattern-specific components of human visual evoked potentials. I. Component of striate cortical origin. Experimental Brain Research, 16( 1), 1-21. |
[27] | Jeffreys, D.A., & Axford, J.G . ( 1972b). Source locations of pattern-specific components of human visual evoked potentials. II. Component of extrastriate cortical origin. Experimental Brain Research, 16( 1), 22-40. |
[28] | Jin, H., Xu, G. P., Zhang, J. X., Ye, Z., Wang, S. F., Zhao, L., ... Mo, L. ( 2010). Athletic training in badminton players modulates the early C1 component of visual evoked potentials: A preliminary investigation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 78( 3), 308-314. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.09.005URL |
[29] | Karns, C.M., & Knight, R.T . ( 2009). Intermodal auditory, visual, and tactile attention modulates early stages of neural processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21( 4), 669-683. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21037URL |
[30] | Kelly, S. P., Gomez-Ramirez, M., & Foxe, J. J. ( 2008). Spatial attention modulates initial afferent activity in human primary visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 18( 11), 2629-2636. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn022URL |
[31] | Kelly, S. P., Vanegas, M. I., Schroeder, C. E., & Lalor, E. C. ( 2013). The cruciform model of striate generation of the early VEP, re-illustrated, not revoked: A reply to Ales et al. (2013). Neuroimage, 82, 154-159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.112URL |
[32] | Khoe, W., Mitchell, J. F., Reynolds, J. H., & Hillyard, S. A. ( 2005). Exogenous attentional selection of transparent superimposed surfaces modulates early event-related potentials. Vision Research, 45( 24), 3004-3014. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.04.021URL |
[33] | Kok, P., Rahnev, D., Jehee, J. F. M., Lau, H. C., & De Lange, F. P..( 2012). Attention reverses the effect of prediction in silencing sensory signals. Cerebral Cortex, 22( 9), 2197-2206. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr310URL |
[34] | Lavie, N.( 1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 21( 3), 451-468. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.451URL |
[35] | Lavie, N., & Tsal, Y.( 1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual-attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 56( 2), 183-197. doi: 10.3758/BF03213897URLpmid: 7971119 |
[36] | Luck, S. J. ( 2005). The Design and interpretation of ERP experiments. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Eds) An introduction to the event-relatedpotential technique. (51-98). Massachusetts: MIT Press,. |
[37] | Martínez, A., Anllo-Vento, L., Sereno, M. I., Frank, L. R., Buxton, R. B., Dubowitz, D. J., ... Hillyard, S. A. ( 1999). Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 364-369. doi: 10.1038/7274URL |
[38] | Martínez, A., DiRusso, F., Anllo-Vento, L., Sereno, M. I., Buxton, R. B., & Hillyard, S. A. ( 2001a). Putting spatial attention on the map: Timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas. Vision Research, 41( 10-11), 1437-1457. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00267-4URL |
[39] | Motter, B.C. ( 1993). Focal attention produces spatially seletive processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. Journal of Neurophysiology, 70( 3), 909-919. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.3.909URL |
[40] | Müller, H.J., & Rabbitt, P. M.A . ( 1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Timecourse of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 15( 2), 315-330. |
[41] | Poghosyan, V., & Ioannides, A.A. ( 2008). Attention modulates earliest responses in the primary auditory and visual cortices. Neuron, 58( 5), 802-813. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.013URL |
[42] | Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. ( 2004). Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 14( 6), 619-633. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh023URL |
[43] | Rauss, K. S., Pourtois, G., Vuilleumier, P., & Schwartz, S. ( 2009). Attentional load modifies early activity in human primary visual cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 30( 5), 1723-1733. doi: 10.1002/hbm.v30:5URL |
[44] | Rauss, K. S., Pourtois, G., Vuilleumier, P., & Schwartz, S. ( 2012a). Effects of attentional load on early visual processing depend on stimulus timing. Human Brain Mapping, 33( 1), 63-74. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21193URL |
[45] | Rauss, K. S., Pourtois, G., Vuilleumier, P., & Schwartz, S. ( 2012b). Voluntary attention reliably influences visual processing at the level of the C1 component: A commentary on Fu, Fedota, Greenwood, and Parasuram (2010). Biological Psychology, 91( 2), 325-327. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.013URL |
[46] | Slagter, H. A., Alilovic, J., & Van Gaal, S. ( 2017). How early does attention modulate visual information processing? The importance of experimental protocol and data analysis approach. Cognitive Neuroscience, 9( 1-2), 26-28. |
[47] | Slotnick, S.D. ( 2013). The nature of attentional modulation in V1. In S. Slotnick (Eds.), Controversies in Cognitive Neuroscience ( pp. 44-69). New York, NY:Palgrave Macmillan. |
[48] | Slotnick, S.D. ( 2018). The experimental parameters that affect attentional modulation of the ERP C1 component. Cognitive Neuroscience, 9( 1-2), 53-62. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2017.1369021URL |
[49] | St?ni?or, L., Van Der Togt, C., Pennartz, C. M. A., & Roelfsema, P. R. ( 2013). A unified selection signal for attention and reward in primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110( 22), 9136-9141. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1300117110URL |
[50] | Sylvester, C. M., Shulman, G. L., Jack, A. I., & Corbetta, M. ( 2009). Anticipatory and stimulus-evoked blood oxygenation level-dependent modulations related to spatial attention reflect a common additive signal. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29( 34), 10671-10682. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1141-09.2009URL |
[51] | Watanabe, M., Cheng, K., Murayama, Y., Ueno, K., Asamizuya, T., Tanaka, K., & Logothetis, N. ( 2011). Attention but not awareness modulates the BOLD signal in the human V1 during binocular suppression. Science, 334( 6057), 829-831. doi: 10.1126/science.1203161URL |
[52] | Woldorff, M. G., Fox, P. T., Matzke, M., Lancaster, J. L., Veeraswamy, S., Zamarripa, F., ... Jerabek, P. ( 1997). Retinotopic organization of early visual spatial attention effects as revealed by PET and ERPs. Human Brain Mapping, 5( 4), 280-286. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1997)5:4<>1.0.CO;2-TURL |
[53] | Zani, A., & Proverbio, A.M. ( 2003). Chapter 1 - Cognitive electrophysiology of mind and brain. Cognitive Electrophysiology of Mind & Brain, 3-12. |
[54] | Zhang, X. L., Zhao, P. L., Zhou, T. G., & Fang, F. ( 2012). Neural activities in V1 create a bottom-up saliency map. Neuron, 73( 1), 183-192. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.035URLpmid: 22243756 |
[55] | Zhu, X.R., & Luo, Y.J . ( 2012). Fearful faces evoke a larger C1 than happy faces in executive attention task: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 526( 2), 118-121. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.08.011URL |
相关文章 15
[1] | 何嘉梅, 金磊. 目标概念的辨析及其对决策的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1410-1419. |
[2] | 荆伟, 张婕, 付锦霞, 田琳, 赵微. 婴幼儿面孔注意偏向:先天倾向与发展轨迹——来自正常和孤独症婴幼儿的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1216-1230. |
[3] | 任筱宇, 赵婧, 毕鸿燕. 动作视频游戏对发展性阅读障碍者阅读技能的影响及其内在机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 1000-1009. |
[4] | 孙国晓, 张力为. 竞赛压力、注意控制与运动表现关系的理论演进[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 1122-1130. |
[5] | 白玉, 杨海波. 创伤后应激障碍个体对威胁刺激的注意偏向:眼动研究的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(4): 737-746. |
[6] | 章小丹, 张沥今, 丁玉珑, 曲折. 注意过程中的行为振荡现象[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(3): 460-471. |
[7] | 霍鹏辉, 冯成志, 陈庭继. 注视者及观察者因素对注视知觉的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(2): 238-251. |
[8] | 张帆, 陈艾睿, 董波, 王爱君, 张明. 视觉注意捕获的快速脱离假说与信号抑制假说[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(1): 45-55. |
[9] | 刘启鹏, 赵小云, 王翠艳, 徐艺雅, 王淑燕. 反刍思维与注意脱离损坏的关系及其神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(1): 102-111. |
[10] | 雷铭, 李朋波. 不同注意形式调节听感觉门控的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(8): 1232-1245. |
[11] | 范晓壮, 毕小彬, 谢宇, 贺荟中. 高功能自闭症个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(7): 1172-1186. |
[12] | 黄子立, 丁玉珑, 曲折. 特征注意的全局性调制作用——增强还是抑制?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(4): 566-578. |
[13] | 尤媛, 王莉. 儿童行为抑制性与心理障碍关联的认知神经过程[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(4): 612-625. |
[14] | 贺淇, 王海英. 冥想对注意能力的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(2): 284-293. |
[15] | 冉光明, 李睿, 张琪. 高社交焦虑者识别动态情绪面孔的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(12): 1979-1988. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4497