删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

分析思维降低情感预测影响偏差

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

耿晓伟(), 刘丹, 牛燕华
鲁东大学教育科学学院, 山东 烟台 264011
收稿日期:2019-07-04出版日期:2020-10-25发布日期:2020-08-24
通讯作者:耿晓伟E-mail:fengandwei@126.com

基金资助:* 国家自然科学基金项目(71401068);国家自然科学基金项目(71971104);教育部人文社科一般项目(19YJA190002);山东省高等学校青创科技支持计划(2019RWF001)

Analytical thinking reduces impact bias in affective forecast

GENG Xiaowei(), LIU Dan, NIU Yanhua
School of Education Science, Ludong University, Yantai 264011, China
Received:2019-07-04Online:2020-10-25Published:2020-08-24
Contact:GENG Xiaowei E-mail:fengandwei@126.com






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 人们在决策前需要对决策可能带来的结果进行预测。人们往往会高估未来事件对情感的影响, 这被称为影响偏差。本研究从双系统理论出发, 考察了分析思维是否会降低情感预测影响偏差。实验1(采用图片启动)和实验2(采用语言流畅性任务)考察了分析思维对影响偏差的影响, 并分析了情感预测程度的中介作用。实验3在现场中以真实的决策(生育二孩)为例, 考察了分析思维启动对情感预测的影响。结果发现:分析思维会降低情感预测强度, 进而降低影响偏差。



图1接受/拒绝条件下启动分析思维对情感预测影响偏差的影响
图1接受/拒绝条件下启动分析思维对情感预测影响偏差的影响



图2接受条件下情感预测强度在启动分析思维对影响偏差影响中的中介作用 注:启动分析思维为虚拟变量, 0 = 控制组, 1 = 启动组。
图2接受条件下情感预测强度在启动分析思维对影响偏差影响中的中介作用 注:启动分析思维为虚拟变量, 0 = 控制组, 1 = 启动组。



图3拒绝条件下情感预测强度在启动分析思维对影响偏差影响中的中介作用 注:启动分析思维为虚拟变量, 0 = 控制组, 1 = 启动组。
图3拒绝条件下情感预测强度在启动分析思维对影响偏差影响中的中介作用 注:启动分析思维为虚拟变量, 0 = 控制组, 1 = 启动组。


表1不同实验条件下消极情感预测、情感体验、影响偏差的独立样本t检验
因变量 启动组 控制组 t df p 95%置信区间 d
情感预测 2.19 ± 0.81 2.78 ± 0.99 -2.36 50 0.02 [-1.09, -0.87] 0.65
情感体验 1.94 ± 0.54 2.09 ± 1.09 -0.65 50 0.52 [-0.63, 0.33] 0.17
影响偏差 0.26 ± 0.65 0.69 ± 0.83 -2.11 50 0.04 [-0.85, -0.02] 0.58

表1不同实验条件下消极情感预测、情感体验、影响偏差的独立样本t检验
因变量 启动组 控制组 t df p 95%置信区间 d
情感预测 2.19 ± 0.81 2.78 ± 0.99 -2.36 50 0.02 [-1.09, -0.87] 0.65
情感体验 1.94 ± 0.54 2.09 ± 1.09 -0.65 50 0.52 [-0.63, 0.33] 0.17
影响偏差 0.26 ± 0.65 0.69 ± 0.83 -2.11 50 0.04 [-0.85, -0.02] 0.58



图4情感预测强度在启动分析思维对影响偏差影响中的中介作用 注:启动分析思维为虚拟变量, 0 = 控制组, 1 = 启动组。
图4情感预测强度在启动分析思维对影响偏差影响中的中介作用 注:启动分析思维为虚拟变量, 0 = 控制组, 1 = 启动组。


表2启动分析思维对积极和消极情感预测的独立样本t检验
因变量 分析思维 情感预测 t df p 95%置信区间 d
积极情感 分析思维 4.54 ± 1.63 2.02 106 0.046 [0.01, 1.27] 0.39
控制组 5.18 ± 1.66
消极情感 分析思维 2.98 ± 1.32 -1.01 106 0.314 [-0.86, 0.28] -0.19
控制组 2.69 ± 1.65

表2启动分析思维对积极和消极情感预测的独立样本t检验
因变量 分析思维 情感预测 t df p 95%置信区间 d
积极情感 分析思维 4.54 ± 1.63 2.02 106 0.046 [0.01, 1.27] 0.39
控制组 5.18 ± 1.66
消极情感 分析思维 2.98 ± 1.32 -1.01 106 0.314 [-0.86, 0.28] -0.19
控制组 2.69 ± 1.65







[1] Eggleston, C. M., Wilson, T. D., Lee, M., & Gilbert, D. T. (2015). Predicting what we will like: Asking a stranger can be as good as asking a friend. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 128, 1-10.
[2] Emanuel, A. S., Updegraff, J. A., Kalmbach, D. A., & Ciesla, J. A. (2010). The role of mindfulness facets in affective forecasting. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(7), 815-818.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.06.012URL
[3] Evans, J. S. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(10), 454-459.
URLpmid: 14550493
[4] Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 255-278.
[5] Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.
URLpmid: 17695343
[6] Franks, A. S., & Scherr, K. C. (2017). Analytic thinking reduces anti-atheist bias in voting intentions. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 27(3), 129-140.
[7] Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25-42.
[8] Geng, X. W., & Jiang, H. Y. (2017). Influence of regulatory focus and regulatory fit on impact biases in affective forecast. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(12), 1537-1547.
[ 耿晓伟, 姜宏艺. (2017). 调节定向和调节匹配对情感预测中影响偏差的影响. 心理学报, 49(12), 1537-1547.]
[9] Geng, X. W., & Zhang, F.(2015). The impact bias in affective forecasting: Focalism or unforeseen adaptation? Journal of Psychological Science, 38(5), 1201-1206.
[ 耿晓伟, 张峰. (2015). 情感预测的影响偏差——聚焦错觉还是适应忽视? 心理科学, 38(5), 1201-1206.]
[10] Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science, 336(6080), 493-496.
doi: 10.1126/science.1215647URLpmid: 22539725
[11] Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 75(3), 617-638.
URLpmid: 9781405
[12] Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science, 317(5843), 1351-1354.
doi: 10.1126/science.1144161URLpmid: 17823345
[13] Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2009). Why the brain talks to itself: Sources of error in emotional prediction. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1335-1341.
[14] Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression- based approach. New York: Guilford.
[15] Hoerger, M., Quirk, S. W., Lucas, R. E., & Carr, T. H. (2010). Cognitive determinants of affective forecasting errors. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(5), 365-373.
URLpmid: 21912580
[16] Hong, P. M., Lishner, D. A., Vogels, E. A., & Ebert, A. R. (2016). The effect of a mindfulness practice and dispositional mindfulness on affective forecasting. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 38(3), 153-165.
[17] Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. American Economic Review, 93(5), 1449-1475.
[18] Kong, D. T. (2015). The role of mindfulness and neuroticism in predicting acculturative anxiety forecasting error. Mindfulness, 6(6), 1387-1400.
[19] Lang, Z., Li, S., Li, Y. M., & Liu, C. J. (2007). Impact bias in happiness prediction. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 21(10), 693-695.
[ 梁哲, 李纾, 李岩梅, 刘长江. (2007). 幸福感预测中的影响偏差. 中国心理卫生杂志, 21(10), 693-695.]
[20] Morewedge, C. K., & Buechel, E. C. (2013). Motivated underpinnings of the impact bias in affective forecasts. Emotion, 13(6), 1023-1029.
[21] Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 934-960.
URLpmid: 12405138
[22] Schwarz, N., & Xu, J. (2011). Why don't we learn from poor choices? The consistency of expectation, choice, and memory clouds the lessons of experience. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(2), 142-145.
[23] Stagnaro, M. N., Ross, R. M., Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2019). Cross-cultural support for a link between analytic thinking and disbelief in God: Evidence from India and the United Kingdom. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(2), 179-186.
[24] Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220-247.
doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1URLpmid: 15454347
[25] Sun, Y., Li, S., & Yin, X. L. (2007). Two systems in decision-making and reasoning: Heuristic system and analytic system. Advances in Psychological Science, 15(5), 721-845.
[ 孙彦, 李纾, 殷晓莉. (2007). 决策与推理的双系统——启发式系统和分析系统. 心理科学进展, 15(5), 721-845.]
[26] Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133(3), 572-585.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006URLpmid: 25217762
[27] Ubel, P. A., Loewenstein, G., Hershey, J., Baron, J., Mohr, T., Asch, D. A., & Jepson, C. (2001). Do nonpatients underestimate the quality of life associated with chronic health conditions because of a focusing illusion? Medical Decision Making, 21(3), 190-199.
URLpmid: 11386626
[28] Ubel, P. A., Loewenstein, G., & Jepson, C. (2005). Disability and sunshine: Can hedonic predictions be improved by drawing attention to focusing illusions or emotional adaptation? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11(2), 111-123.
[29] Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 345-411). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
[30] Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 821-836.
[31] Wirtz, D., Kruger, J., Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2003). What to do on spring break? The role of predicted, on-line, and remembered experience in future choive. Psychological Science, 14(5), 520-524.
[32] Xu, J., &Jiang, D. (2009). Expectations, experiences, and memories: When consumers fail to learn from experiences. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 41(8), 745-752.
[ 徐菁, 蒋多. (2009). 期望、体验和回忆:当消费者不能从体验中学习. 心理学报, 41(8), 745-752.]
[33] Yilmaz, O., Karad?ller, D. Z., & Sofuoglu, G. (2016). Analytic thinking, religion and prejudice: An experimental test of the dual-process model of mind. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 26(4), 360-369.
[34] Zhang, Y. P. (2005). Evaluations on ultimatum game. Economic Perspectives,(6), 83-86.
[ 张元鹏. (2005). 最后通牒博弈实验及其评价. 经济学动态, (6), 83-86.]




[1]黄敏学,王薇. 移动购物更快吗?决策场景与思维模式的相容性[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(5): 612-624.
[2]耿晓伟, 张峰, 王艳净, 范琳琳, 姚艳. 健康目标启动降低高热量食物消费[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(8): 840-847.
[3]耿晓伟, 姜宏艺. 调节定向和调节匹配对情感预测中 影响偏差的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(12): 1537-1547.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4805
相关话题/心理 实验 检验 科学 系统