1 浙江工商大学工商管理学院
2 浙江工商大学浙商研究院
3 浙江工商大学MBA学院, 杭州 310018
4 南开大学泽尔滕实验室, 南开大学滨海学院, 天津 300071
5 浙江工商大学财务与会计学院, 杭州 310018
收稿日期:
2018-08-10出版日期:
2019-05-25发布日期:
2019-03-20通讯作者:
李建标,郝洁E-mail:biaojl@126.com;haojie@mail.zjgsu.edu.cn基金资助:
浙江省高校人文社会科学重点研究基地(浙江工商大学工商管理学科)(JYTgs20181107);国家自然科学基金(71673152)Neural mechanisms of third-party punishment: Evidence from transcranial direct current stimulation
YIN Xile1,2, LI Jianbiao3,4(), CHEN Siyu1, LIU Xiaoli4, HAO Jie5()1 School of Business Administration, Zhejiang Gongshang University, Hangzhou 310018, China
2 Zheshang Research Institute, Zhejiang Gongshang University, Hangzhou 310018, China
3 MBA School, Zhejiang Gongshang University, Hangzhou 310018, China
4 Selten Laboratory, Binhai College, Nankai University, Tianjin 300071, China
5 School of Accounting, Zhejiang Gongshang University, Hangzhou 310018, China
Received:
2018-08-10Online:
2019-05-25Published:
2019-03-20Contact:
LI Jianbiao,HAO Jie E-mail:biaojl@126.com;haojie@mail.zjgsu.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 第三方惩罚既是社会规范在群体得以维系的基石, 也是个体维护社会规范的体现。当前关注社会规范的神经研究大多基于第二方惩罚的独裁者或最后通牒实验框架, 缺乏对第三方维护社会规范过程中相关脑区活动的探索, 对这一过程的内在神经机制也不清楚。本文基于第三方惩罚的独裁者博弈框架, 对右侧背外侧前额叶区域(DLPFC)进行不同极性的经颅直流电刺激(tDCS), 同时依据第三方是否需要为其惩罚付出成本设计了零成本和有成本两个实验任务。结果发现, 第三方在零成本任务的情绪反应和惩罚显著受到tDCS设置的影响, 且阴极刺激显著提升了第三方的惩罚值, 这表明情绪机制对第三方惩罚有着重要影响。另外, 第三方在零成本和有成本任务中的惩罚差异在不同tDCS设置之间也存在显著差异, 这与第三方惩罚还受到自利机制影响的观点相符。本文率先为右侧DLPFC活动影响第三方惩罚提供了神经层面的证据, 且支持了第三方对社会规范的遵从与其负性情绪反应和自利加工密切相关的机制解释。
图/表 6
图1HD-Explore软件绘制的电流模式展示(左图为阳极刺激, 右图为阴极刺激)
图1HD-Explore软件绘制的电流模式展示(左图为阳极刺激, 右图为阴极刺激)
图2独裁者A的决策界面
图2独裁者A的决策界面
图3不同分配方案下第三方的情绪及其惩罚值(误差线为标准误)
图3不同分配方案下第三方的情绪及其惩罚值(误差线为标准误)
表1独裁者博弈中第三方情绪反应的描述性统计
方案公平性 | tDCS | n | M | SD | ANOVA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
不公平 | 阴极 | 29 | -1.38 | 0.78 | F(2, 86) = 12.07, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.219 |
虚拟 | 30 | -0.97 | 0.72 | ||
阳极 | 30 | -0.33 | 0.96 | ||
总体 | 89 | -0.89 | 0.92 | ||
中等 | 阴极 | 29 | 0.00 | 0.80 | F(2, 86) = 0.83, p = 0.438 |
虚拟 | 30 | 0.23 | 0.77 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 0.20 | 0.66 | ||
总体 | 89 | 0.15 | 0.75 | ||
公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 1.52 | 0.57 | F(2, 86) = 1.43, p = 0.245 |
虚拟 | 30 | 1.30 | 0.70 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 1.23 | 0.73 | ||
总体 | 89 | 1.35 | 0.68 |
表1独裁者博弈中第三方情绪反应的描述性统计
方案公平性 | tDCS | n | M | SD | ANOVA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
不公平 | 阴极 | 29 | -1.38 | 0.78 | F(2, 86) = 12.07, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.219 |
虚拟 | 30 | -0.97 | 0.72 | ||
阳极 | 30 | -0.33 | 0.96 | ||
总体 | 89 | -0.89 | 0.92 | ||
中等 | 阴极 | 29 | 0.00 | 0.80 | F(2, 86) = 0.83, p = 0.438 |
虚拟 | 30 | 0.23 | 0.77 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 0.20 | 0.66 | ||
总体 | 89 | 0.15 | 0.75 | ||
公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 1.52 | 0.57 | F(2, 86) = 1.43, p = 0.245 |
虚拟 | 30 | 1.30 | 0.70 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 1.23 | 0.73 | ||
总体 | 89 | 1.35 | 0.68 |
表2独裁者博弈中第三方惩罚值的描述性统计(零成本设置)
方案公平性 | tDCS | n | M | SD | ANOVA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
不公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 29.59 | 9.88 | F(2, 86) = 9.29, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.178 |
虚拟 | 30 | 19.37 | 13.74 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 16.23 | 13.19 | ||
总体 | 89 | 21.64 | 13.54 | ||
中等 | 阴极 | 29 | 9.69 | 8.61 | F(2, 86) = 1.43, p = 0.246 |
虚拟 | 30 | 6.53 | 6.75 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 6.50 | 9.35 | ||
总体 | 89 | 7.55 | 8.35 | ||
公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 1.76 | 9.28 | F(2, 86) = 0.07, p = 0.935 |
虚拟 | 30 | 1.83 | 9.14 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 1.17 | 3.13 | ||
总体 | 89 | 1.58 | 7.63 |
表2独裁者博弈中第三方惩罚值的描述性统计(零成本设置)
方案公平性 | tDCS | n | M | SD | ANOVA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
不公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 29.59 | 9.88 | F(2, 86) = 9.29, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.178 |
虚拟 | 30 | 19.37 | 13.74 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 16.23 | 13.19 | ||
总体 | 89 | 21.64 | 13.54 | ||
中等 | 阴极 | 29 | 9.69 | 8.61 | F(2, 86) = 1.43, p = 0.246 |
虚拟 | 30 | 6.53 | 6.75 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 6.50 | 9.35 | ||
总体 | 89 | 7.55 | 8.35 | ||
公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 1.76 | 9.28 | F(2, 86) = 0.07, p = 0.935 |
虚拟 | 30 | 1.83 | 9.14 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 1.17 | 3.13 | ||
总体 | 89 | 1.58 | 7.63 |
表3独裁者博弈中第三方惩罚差异的描述性统计
方案公平性 | tDCS | n | M | SD | ANOVA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
不公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 21.90 | 13.10 | F(2, 86) = 6.62, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.133 |
虚拟 | 30 | 12.67 | 12.92 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 10.10 | 13.17 | ||
总体 | 89 | 14.81 | 13.87 | ||
中等 | 阴极 | 29 | 7.48 | 9.48 | F(2, 86) = 1.7, p = 0.188 |
虚拟 | 30 | 4.00 | 6.68 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 3.07 | 12.07 | ||
总体 | 89 | 4.82 | 9.74 | ||
公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 1.76 | 9.28 | F(2, 86) = 0.08, p = 0.925 |
虚拟 | 30 | 1.33 | 9.28 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 0.97 | 2.65 | ||
总体 | 89 | 1.35 | 7.63 |
表3独裁者博弈中第三方惩罚差异的描述性统计
方案公平性 | tDCS | n | M | SD | ANOVA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
不公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 21.90 | 13.10 | F(2, 86) = 6.62, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.133 |
虚拟 | 30 | 12.67 | 12.92 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 10.10 | 13.17 | ||
总体 | 89 | 14.81 | 13.87 | ||
中等 | 阴极 | 29 | 7.48 | 9.48 | F(2, 86) = 1.7, p = 0.188 |
虚拟 | 30 | 4.00 | 6.68 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 3.07 | 12.07 | ||
总体 | 89 | 4.82 | 9.74 | ||
公平 | 阴极 | 29 | 1.76 | 9.28 | F(2, 86) = 0.08, p = 0.925 |
虚拟 | 30 | 1.33 | 9.28 | ||
阳极 | 30 | 0.97 | 2.65 | ||
总体 | 89 | 1.35 | 7.63 |
参考文献 50
1 | Chen, S. J., & Ma, J. H . ( 2011). Third-party punishment and social norm activation: The influence of social responsibility and emotion. Journal of Psychological Science, 34( 3), 670-675. |
[ 陈思静, 马剑虹 . ( 2011). 第三方惩罚与社会规范激活——社会责任感与情绪的作用. 心理科学, 34( 3), 670-675.] | |
2 | Chen S. J., He Q., & Ma J. H . ( 2015). The influence of third- party punishment on cooperation: An explanation of social norm activation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47( 3), 389-405. |
[ 陈思静, 何铨, 马剑虹 . ( 2015). 第三方惩罚对合作行为的影响: 基于社会规范激活的解释. 心理学报, 47( 3), 389-405.] | |
3 | Cialdini, R. B., & Trost, , M. R . ( 1998). Social influence: Social norms, conformity and compliance. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology ( pp. 1-17). New York, NY, US: McGraw-Hill. |
4 | Civai C., Miniussi C., & Rumiati R. I . ( 2015). Medial prefrontal cortex reacts to unfairness if this damages the self: A tDCS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10( 8), 1054-1060. |
5 | Cohen, J. ( 1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. |
6 | Corradi-Dell'Acqua C., Civai C., Rumiati R. I., & Fink G. R . ( 2012). Disentangling self- and fairness-related neural mechanisms involved in the ultimatum game: An fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8( 4), 424-431. |
7 | de la Fuente-Fernández R., Ruth T. J., Sossi V., Schulzer M., Calne D. B., & Stoessl A. J . ( 2001). Expectation and dopamine release: Mechanism of the placebo effect in Parkinson's disease. Science, 293( 5532), 1164-1166. |
8 | Elster, J. ( 1989). Social norms and economic theory. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 3( 4), 99-117. |
9 | Fan L. C., Lu L., & Liang J . ( 2013). The demand for the third party punishment: An Experimental Examination. Economic Research Journal, ( 5), 98-111. |
[ 范良聪, 刘璐, 梁捷 . ( 2013). 第三方的惩罚需求: 一个实验研究. 经济研究, ( 5), 98-111.] | |
10 | Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U . ( 2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25( 2), 63-87. |
11 | Fehr, E., & Gächter, S . ( 2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415( 6868), 137-140. |
12 | Feinstein J. S., Adolphs R., Damasio A., & Tranel D . ( 2011). The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear. Current Biology, 21( 1), 34-38. |
13 | Feng C., Luo Y-J., & Krueger F . ( 2015). Neural signatures of fairness-related normative decision making in the ultimatum game: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 36( 2), 591-602. |
14 | Filmer H. L., Dux P. E., & Mattingley J. B . ( 2014). Applications of transcranial direct current stimulation for understanding brain function. Trends in Neurosciences, 37( 12), 742-753. |
15 | Filmer H. L., Mattingley J. B., & Dux P. E . ( 2013). Improved multitasking following prefrontal tDCS. Cortex, 49( 10), 2845-2852. |
16 | Fischbacher, U. ( 2007). z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10( 2), 171-178. |
17 | Fischbacher U., Gächter S., & Fehr E . ( 2001). Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 71( 3), 397-404. |
18 | Gan T., Li W. Q., Tang H. H., Lu X. P., Li, X. L, Liu, C., & Luo Y. J . ( 2013). Exciting the right temporo-parietal junction with transcranial direct current stimulation influences moral intention processing. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45( 9), 1004-1014. |
[ 甘甜, 李万清, 唐红红, 陆夏平, 李小俚, 刘超, 罗跃嘉 . ( 2013). 经颅直流电刺激右侧颞顶联合区对道德意图加工的影响. 心理学报, 45( 9), 1004-1014.] | |
19 | Gan T., Shi R., Liu C., & Luo Y. J . ( 2018). Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation on the right temporo-parietal junction modulates the helpful intention processing. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50( 1), 36-46. |
[ 甘甜, 石睿, 刘超, 罗跃嘉 . ( 2018). 经颅直流电刺激右侧颞顶联合区对助人意图加工的影响. 心理学报, 50( 1), 36-46.] | |
20 | Gospic K., Mohlin E., Fransson P., Petrovic P., Johannesson M., & Ingvar M . ( 2011). Limbic justice--Amygdala involvement in immediate rejection in the Ultimatum Game. PLoS Biology, 9( 5), e1001054. |
21 | Harty S., Robertson I. H., Miniussi C., Sheehy O. C., Devine C. A., McCreery S., & O'Connell R. G . ( 2014). Transcranial direct current stimulation over right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex enhances error awareness in older age. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34( 10), 3646-3652. |
22 | Hummel F., Celnik P., Giraux P., Floel A., Wu W.-H., Gerloff C., & Cohen L. G . ( 2005). Effects of non-invasive cortical stimulation on skilled motor function in chronic stroke. Brain, 128( 3), 490-499. |
23 | Jacobson L., Koslowsky M., & Lavidor M . ( 2012). tDCS polarity effects in motor and cognitive domains: A meta- analytical review. Experimental Brain Research, 216( 1), 1-10. |
24 | Jordan J., Mcauliffe K., & Rand D . ( 2016). The effects of endowment size and strategy method on third party punishment. Experimental Economics, 19( 4), 741-763. |
25 | Knoch D., Nitsche M. A., Fischbacher U., Eisenegger C., Pascual-Leone A., & Fehr E . ( 2008). Studying the neurobiology of social interaction with transcranial direct current stimulation—The example of punishing unfairness. Cerebral Cortex, 18( 9), 1987-1990. |
26 | Knoch D., Pascual-Leone A., Meyer K., Treyer V., & Fehr E . ( 2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science, 314( 5800), 829-832. |
27 | Li J., Yin X., Li D., Liu X., Wang G., & Qu L . ( 2017). Controlling the anchoring effect through transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1079. |
28 | Luo J., Ye H., Zheng H. L., Jia Y. M., Chen S., & Huang D. Q . ( 2017). Modulating the activities of right and left temporo-parietal junction influences the capability of moral intention processing: A transcranial direct current stimulation study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49( 2), 228-240. |
[ 罗俊, 叶航, 郑昊力, 贾拥民, 陈姝, 黄达强 . ( 2017). 左右侧颞顶联合区对道德意图信息加工能力的共同作用——基于经颅直流电刺激技术. 心理学报, 49( 2), 228-240.] | |
29 | Luo Y., Feng C. L., Gu R. L., Wu T. T., & Luo Y. J . ( 2013). The fairness norm in social decision-making: Behavioral and neuroscience studies. Advances in Psychological Science, 21( 2), 300-308. |
[ 罗艺, 封春亮, 古若雷, 吴婷婷, 罗跃嘉 . ( 2013). 社会决策中的公平准则及其神经机制. 心理科学进展, 21( 2), 300-308.] | |
30 | Meiron, O., & Lavidor, M . ( 2013). Unilateral prefrontal direct current stimulation effects are modulated by working memory load and gender. Brain Stimulation, 6( 3), 440-447. |
31 | Nitsche, M. A., & Paulus, W . ( 2001). Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology, 57( 10), 1899-1901. |
32 | Ochsner K. N., Ray R. D., Cooper J. C., Robertson E. R., Chopra S., Gabrieli J. D., & Gross J. J . ( 2004). For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cognitive down-and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage, 23( 2), 483-499. |
33 | Ravizza, S. M., & Carter, C. S . ( 2008). Shifting set about task switching: Behavioral and neural evidence for distinct forms of cognitive flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 46( 12), 2924-2935. |
34 | Rêgo G. G., Lapenta O. M., Marques L. M., Costa T. L., Leite J., Carvalho S., .. Boggio P. S . ( 2015). Hemispheric dorsolateral prefrontal cortex lateralization in the regulation of empathy for pain. Neuroscience Letters, 594, 12-16. |
35 | Reno R. R., Cialdini R. B., & Kallgren C. A . ( 1993). The transsituational influence of social norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64( 1), 104-112. |
36 | Rowe J. B., Sakai K., Lund T. E., Ramsøy T., Christensen M. S., Baare W. F. C., .. Passingham R. E . ( 2007). Is the prefrontal cortex necessary for establishing cognitive sets? Journal of Neuroscience, 27( 48), 13303-13310. |
37 | Ruff C. C., Ugazio G., & Fehr E . ( 2013). Changing social norm compliance with noninvasive brain stimulation. Science, 342( 6157), 482-484. |
38 | Sakai, K. ( 2008). Task set and prefrontal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31( 1), 219-245. |
39 | Sanfey A. G., Rilling J. K., Aronson J. A., Nystrom L. E., & Cohen J. D . ( 2003). The neural basis of economic decision- making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300( 5626), 1755-1758. |
40 | Sellaro R., Nitsche M. A., & Colzato L. S . ( 2016). The stimulated social brain: Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on social cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1369( 1), 218-239. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13098URL |
41 | Spitzer M., Fischbacher U., Herrnberger B., Grön G., & Fehr E . ( 2007). The neural signature of social norm compliance. Neuron, 56( 1), 185-196. |
42 | Tremblay S., Lepage J.-F., Latulipe-Loiselle A., Fregni F., Pascual-Leone A., & Théoret H . ( 2014). The uncertain outcome of prefrontal tDCS. Brain Stimulation, 7( 6), 773-783. |
43 | Wang G., Li J., Yin X., Li S., & Wei M . ( 2016). Modulating activity in the orbitofrontal cortex changes trustees' cooperation: A transcranial direct current stimulation study. Behavioural Brain Research, 303( 4), 71-75. |
44 | Wang Y., Wang Y., Lin C., Chen X., Yuan B., & Shen D . ( 2011). Modulation of conscientiousness on medial frontal negativity in negative emotions: An ERP study on ultimatum Game. Scientia Sinica Vitae, 41( 4), 320-331. |
[ 王益文, 王钰, 林崇德, 陈雪莹, 袁博, 沈德立 . ( 2011). 内侧额叶负波受负性情绪下责任感影响: 最后通牒任务的ERP研究. 中国科学: 生命科学, 41( 4), 320-331.] | |
45 | Wang Y., Zhang Z., Zhang W., Huang L., Guo F., & Yuan S . ( 2014). Group membership modulates the recipient’s fairness consideration in ultimatum game. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46( 12), 1850-1859. |
[ 王益文, 张振, 张蔚, 黄亮, 郭丰波, 原胜 . ( 2014). 群体身份调节最后通牒博弈的公平关注. 心理学报, 46( 12), 1850-1859.] | |
46 | Willis M. L., Murphy J. M., Ridley N. J., & Vercammen A . ( 2015). Anodal tDCS targeting the right orbitofrontal cortex enhances facial expression recognition. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10( 12), 1677-1683. |
47 | Wu, Y., & Zhou, X. L . ( 2012). The context-dependency of fairness processing: Evidence from ERP study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44( 6), 797-806. |
[ 吴燕, 周晓林 . ( 2012). 公平加工的情境依赖性: 来自ERP的证据. 心理学报, 44( 6), 797-806.] | |
48 | Ye H., Chen S., Huang D., Wang S., & Luo J . ( 2015). Modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex changes decision-making for risky gains and losses: A transcranial direct current stimulation study. Behavioural Brain Research, 286, 17-21. |
49 | Zhou P. Y., Wang K., Li Q., & Liu X . ( 2012). Neural mechanisms of emotional modulation on memory. Chinese Science Bulletin, 57( 35), 3367-3375. |
[ 周平艳, 王凯, 李琦, 刘勋 . ( 2012). 情绪影响记忆的神经机制. 科学通报, 57( 35), 3367-3375.] | |
50 | Zhou Y., Wang Y., Rao L.-L., Yang L.-Q., & Li S . ( 2014). Money talks: Neural substrate of modulation of fairness by monetary incentives. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 150. |
相关文章 15
[1] | 程瑞, 卢克龙, 郝宁. 愤怒情绪对恶意创造力的影响及调节策略[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 847-860. |
[2] | 宋琪, 陈扬. 需求和接受的授权型领导匹配对下属工作结果的影响:情绪耗竭的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 890-903. |
[3] | 陈思静, 濮雪丽, 朱玥, 汪昊, 刘建伟. 规范错觉对外出就餐中食物浪费的影响: 心理机制与应对策略[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 904-918. |
[4] | 熊承清, 许佳颖, 马丹阳, 刘永芳. 囚徒困境博弈中对手面部表情对合作行为的影响及其作用机制[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 919-932. |
[5] | 陈思静, 邢懿琳, 翁异静, 黎常. 第三方惩罚对合作的溢出效应:基于社会规范的解释[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 758-772. |
[6] | 袁加锦, 张祎程, 陈圣栋, 罗利, 茹怡珊. 中国情绪调节词语库的初步编制与试用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 445-455. |
[7] | 宋锡妍, 程亚华, 谢周秀甜, 龚楠焰, 刘雷. 愤怒情绪对延迟折扣的影响:确定感和控制感的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 456-468. |
[8] | 莫李澄, 郭田友, 张岳瑶, 徐锋, 张丹丹. 激活右腹外侧前额叶提高抑郁症患者对社会疼痛的情绪调节能力:一项TMS研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 494-504. |
[9] | 杨伟文, 李超平. 资质过剩感对个体绩效的作用效果及机制:基于情绪-认知加工系统与文化情境的元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 527-554. |
[10] | 侯娟, 朱英格, 方晓义. 手机成瘾与抑郁:社交焦虑和负性情绪信息注意偏向的多重中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(4): 362-373. |
[11] | 刘宇平, 李姗珊, 何赟, 王豆豆, 杨波. 消除威胁或无能狂怒?自恋对暴力犯攻击的影响机制[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(3): 244-258. |
[12] | 雷震, 毕蓉, 莫李澄, 于文汶, 张丹丹. 外显和内隐情绪韵律加工的脑机制:近红外成像研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(1): 15-25. |
[13] | 黄月胜, 张豹, 范兴华, 黄杰. 无关工作记忆表征的负性情绪信息能否捕获视觉注意?一项眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(1): 26-37. |
[14] | 苗晓燕, 孙欣, 匡仪, 汪祚军. 共患难, 更同盟:共同经历相同负性情绪事件促进合作行为[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(1): 81-94. |
[15] | 华艳, 李明霞, 王巧婷, 冯彩霞, 张晶. 左侧眶额皮层在自动情绪调节下注意选择中的作用:来自经颅直流电刺激的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(9): 1048-1056. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4439