
山东师范大学心理学院, 济南 250014
收稿日期:
2018-11-28出版日期:
2019-11-25发布日期:
2019-09-24通讯作者:
贾世伟E-mail:jiashiwei82@126.com基金资助:
* 国家自然科学基金项目(31200784)资助Local context dependence in feedback evaluation: An ERP study
ZHU Shuqing, ZHAI Yu, JIA Shiwei(
School of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, Jinan 250014, China
Received:
2018-11-28Online:
2019-11-25Published:
2019-09-24Contact:
JIA Shiwei E-mail:jiashiwei82@126.com摘要/Abstract
摘要: 人脑对反馈的评估依赖于其所处背景。但是只能依赖整体背景(整个组块的结果范围), 还是可以依赖局部背景(单个试次的结果范围)进行评估, 目前并不清楚。本研究通过获益/损失线索操纵所在试次的反馈背景, 探讨反馈评估仅依赖整体背景, 还是可延伸到局部水平。25名被试参与实验。结果发现, 背景试次间变化时, 在获益背景下, ¥0 (负反馈)比+ ¥4 (正反馈)引发更负的FRN (feedback-related negativity); 在损失背景下, - ¥4 (负反馈)比¥0 (正反馈)引发更负的FRN。这一结果说明反馈评估以某线索背景中可能的结果为参考对象, FRN的背景依赖可以延伸到局部水平。结合前人研究, 推测任务类型和反馈真实性会调节背景依赖水平。在含真实反馈的主动性任务中, FRN的背景依赖效应可延伸至局部水平。
图/表 6

图1背景依赖情况示意图。如果结果评估采取整体依赖模式, 结果如左图所示, 被试以所有可能的结果为参照, 相对坏的结果比相对好的结果引发更负的FRN, 不受所在试次的线索影响。如果结果评估采取局部依赖模式, 结果如右图所示, 被试以本试次的可能的结果为参照, 相对坏的结果比相对好的结果引发更负的FRN。


图2实验流程示意图


图3时间估计调整量的平均值及标准差 注:* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001


图4四种实验条件下FRN波幅的平均值及标准差(Fz, FCz和Cz点) 注:* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001


图5四种实验条件下的FRN总平均图(Fz、FCz和Cz点)


图6正、负反馈所引发FRN波幅图(Fz点)及其差异波的地形图

参考文献 36
[1] | Angus, D. J., Latham, A. J., Harmon-Jones, E., Deliano, M., Balleine, B., & Braddon-Mitchell, D . (2017). Electrocortical components of anticipation and consumption in a monetary incentive delay task. Psychophysiology, 54(11), 1686-1705. |
[2] | Gehring, W. J., & Willoughby, A. R . (2002). The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science, 295(5563), 2279-2282. |
[3] | Hajcak, G., Holroyd, C. B., Moser, J. S., & Simons, R. F . (2005). Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. Psychophysiology, 42(2), 161-170. |
[4] | Hajcak, G., Moser J. S, Holroyd, C. B., & Simons, R. F . (2006). The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes. Biological Psychology, 71(2), 148-154. |
[5] | Heydari, S., & HolroydC, B . (2016). Reward positivity: Reward prediction error or salience prediction error?. Psychophysiology, 53(8), 1185-1192. |
[6] | Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H . (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679-709. |
[7] | Holroyd, C. B., Hajcak, G., & Larsen, J. T . (2006). The good, the bad and the neutral: Electrophysiological responses to feedback stimuli. Brain Research, 1105(1), 93-101. |
[8] | Holroyd, C. B., Larsen, J. T., & Cohen, J. D . (2004). Context dependence of the event-related brain potential associated with reward and punishment. Psychophysiology, 41(2), 245-253. |
[9] | Holroyd, C. B., Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., & Cohen, J. D . (2003). Errors in reward prediction are reflected in the event-related brain potential. Neuroreport, 14(18), 2481-2484. |
[10] | Kujawa, A., Smith, E., Luhmann, C., & Hajcak, G . (2013). The feedback negativity reflects favorable compared to nonfavorable outcomes based on global, not local, alternatives. Psychophysiology, 50(2), 134-138. |
[11] | Li, D., Li, P., & Li, H . (2018). The updated theories of feedback-related negativity in the last decade. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(9), 1642-1650. |
[12] | [ 李丹阳, 李鹏, 李红 . (2018). 反馈负波及其近10年理论解释. 心理科学进展, 26(9), 1642-1650.] |
[13] | Luft, C. D. B . (2014). Learning from feedback: The neural mechanisms of feedback processing facilitating better performance. Behavioural Brain Research, 261, 356-368. |
[14] | Mars, R. B., De Bruijn, E. R. A.., Hulstijn, W., Miltner, W. H. R Coles, M. G. H.& . (2004). What if I told you: ‘you were wrong’? Brain potentials and behavioral adjustments elicited by feedback in a time-estimation task. M. Ullsperger, M. Falkenstein (Eds.), Errors, Conflicts, and the Brain. Current Opinions on Performance Monitoring, MPI of Cognitive Neuroscience, Leipzig, 129-134. |
[15] | Meadows, C. C., Gable, P. A., Lohse, K. R., & Miller, M. W . (2016). The effects of reward magnitude on reward processing: An averaged and single trial event-related potential study. Biological Psychology, 118, 154-160. |
[16] | Miltner, W. H., Braun, C. H., & Coles, M. G . (1997). Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: Evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(6), 788-798. |
[17] | Mulligan, E. M., & Hajcak, G. .(2018). The electrocortical response to rewarding and aversive feedback: The reward positivity does not reflect salience in simple gambling tasks. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 132, 262-267. |
[18] | Nieuwenhuis, S., Heslenfeld, D. J., von Geusau, N. J. A.., Mars, R. B., Holroyd, C. B., & Yeung, N . (2005). Activity in human reward-sensitive brain areas is strongly context dependent. Neuroimage, 25(4), 1302-1309. |
[19] | Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., Holroyd, C. B., Schurger, A., & Cohen, J. D . (2004). Sensitivity of electrophysiological activity from medial frontal cortex to utilitarian and performance feedback. Cerebral Cortex, 14(7), 741-747. |
[20] | Oldfield, R. C . (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97-113. |
[21] | Osinsky, R., Mussel, P., & Hewig, J . (2012). Feedback-related potentials are sensitive to sequential order of decision outcomes in a gambling task. Psychophysiology, 49(12), 1579-1589 |
[22] | Osinsky, R., Walter, H., & Hewig, J . (2014). What is and what could have been: An ERP study on counterfactual comparisons. Psychophysiology, 51(8), 773-781. |
[23] | Pfabigan, D. M., Alexopoulos, J., Bauer, H., & Sailer, U . (2011). Manipulation of feedback expectancy and valence induces negative and positive reward prediction error signals manifest in event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 48(5), 656-664. |
[24] | Pfabigan, D. M., Seidel, E. M., Paul, K., Grahl, A., Sailer, U Lanzenberger, R.… Lamm, C. (2015). Context-sensitivity of the feedback-related negativity for zero-value feedback outcomes. Biological Psychology, 104, 184-192. |
[25] | Proudfit, G. H . (2015). The reward positivity: From basic research on reward to a biomarker for depression. Psychophysiology, 52(4), 449-459. |
[26] | Sambrook, T. D., & Goslin, J. ,(2015). A neural reward prediction error revealed by a meta-analysis of ERPs using great grand averages. Psychological Bulletin, 141(1), 213-235. |
[27] | Soder, H. E., & Potts, G. F . (2018). Medial frontal cortex response to unexpected motivationally salient outcomes. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 132, 268-276. |
[28] | Talmi, D., Atkinson, R., & El-deredy, W . (2013). The feedback-related negativity signals salience prediction errors, not reward prediction errors . Journal of Neuroscience, 33(19), 8264-8269. |
[29] | Ullsperger, M., Danielmeier, C., & Jocham, G . (2014). Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior. Physiological Reviews, 94(1), 35-79. |
[30] | Walsh, M. M.,& Anderson, J. R . (2012). Learning from experience: Event-related potential correlates of reward processing, neural adaptation, and behavioral choice. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(8), 1870-1884. |
[31] | Wang, L. J., Tang, D. D., Zhao, Y. F., Hitchman, G., Wu, S. S., Tan, J. F., & Chen, A. T . (2015). Disentangling the impacts of outcome valence and outcome frequency on the post-error slowing. Scientific Reports, 5, 8708. |
[32] | Xiang, L., Wang, B. X., & Zhang, Q. L . (2012). Performance monitoring and behavioral adjustments in a time- estimation task: Evidence from ERP study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(9), 1149-1159. |
[33] | [ 向玲, 王宝玺, 张庆林 . (2012). 时间估计任务中的反馈加工和行为调节: 来自ERP的证据. 心理学报, 44(9), 1149-1159.] |
[34] | Xiang, L., Wang, B. X., Zhang, Q. L., & Yuan, H . (2008). The effect of reference point of the feedback-related ERPs. Advances in Psychological Science, 40(12), 1240-1249. |
[35] | [ 向玲, 王宝玺, 张庆林, 袁宏 . (2008). 反馈相关ERP成分的参照点效应. 心理学报, 40( 12), 1240-1249.] |
[36] | Yeung, N., & Sanfey, A. G . (2004). Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(28), 6258-6264. |
相关文章 1
[1] | 向玲,王宝玺,张庆林,袁宏. 反馈相关ERP成分的参照点效应[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(12): 1240-1249. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4554