
浙江师范大学心理学系, 金华 321004
收稿日期:
2018-11-19出版日期:
2019-08-21发布日期:
2019-07-24通讯作者:
金心怡E-mail:jinxinyi@zjnu.edu.cn基金资助:
* 浙江省自然科学基金青年项目(LQ19C090004)Level 2 visual perspective-taking at age 3 and the corresponding effect of cooperation
JIN Xinyi(
Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua 321004, China
Received:
2018-11-19Online:
2019-08-21Published:
2019-07-24Contact:
JIN Xinyi E-mail:jinxinyi@zjnu.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 研究以经典的乌龟任务(实验1)和改良的滤镜任务(实验2)对3岁幼儿的二级观点采择进行探究, 并探讨其在短暂的社会互动前后的变化。结果表明:(1) 3岁幼儿尚难以自发进行二级观点采择, 但已具备进行这一加工的潜质; (2)短暂的合作互动可在一定程度上提高幼儿采择他人观点的成功率, 而竞争互动则无类似影响。该发现为从建构主义角度解释视觉观点采择发展提供了新的证据。
图/表 6

图1社会互动游戏“钓鱼游戏”


图2实验1“乌龟任务”场景示意图

表1实验1“乌龟任务”被试得分情况
组别 | 得分 | 前测 | 后测 |
---|---|---|---|
合作组 | 1 | 3 (12.50%) | 8 (33.33%) |
0 | 21 (87.50%) | 16 (66.67%) | |
竞争组 | 1 | 3 (12.50%) | 3 (12.50%) |
0 | 21 (87.50%) | 21 (87.50%) |
表1实验1“乌龟任务”被试得分情况
组别 | 得分 | 前测 | 后测 |
---|---|---|---|
合作组 | 1 | 3 (12.50%) | 8 (33.33%) |
0 | 21 (87.50%) | 16 (66.67%) | |
竞争组 | 1 | 3 (12.50%) | 3 (12.50%) |
0 | 21 (87.50%) | 21 (87.50%) |

图3实验2a与2b“滤镜任务”场景示意图

表2实验2a“滤镜任务”被试得分情况
组别 | 前后测 | 得分情况 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
2 | 1 | 0 | ||
合作组 | 前测 | 7 (29.17%) | 13 (54.17%) | 4 (16.67%) |
后测 | 15 (62.50%) | 9 (37.50%) | 0 (0.00%) | |
竞争组 | 前测 | 8 (33.33%) | 13 (54.17%) | 3 (12.50%) |
后测 | 7 (29.17%) | 13 (54.17%) | 4 (16.67%) |
表2实验2a“滤镜任务”被试得分情况
组别 | 前后测 | 得分情况 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
2 | 1 | 0 | ||
合作组 | 前测 | 7 (29.17%) | 13 (54.17%) | 4 (16.67%) |
后测 | 15 (62.50%) | 9 (37.50%) | 0 (0.00%) | |
竞争组 | 前测 | 8 (33.33%) | 13 (54.17%) | 3 (12.50%) |
后测 | 7 (29.17%) | 13 (54.17%) | 4 (16.67%) |
表3实验2b及Moll & Meltzoff (2011)二级观点采择得分对比
“颜色混合”任务数据来源 | 得分情况 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 | 1 | 0 | |||||
实验2b | 36月以下组(n = 11) | 11 (45.83%) | 5 (45.45%) | 5 (20.83%) | 2 (18.18%) | 8 (33.33%) | 4 (36.36%) |
36月及以上组(n = 13) | 6 (46.15%) | 3 (23.08%) | 4 (30.77%) | ||||
Moll & | 15 (62.50%) | 8 (33.33%) | 1 (4.17%) |
表3实验2b及Moll & Meltzoff (2011)二级观点采择得分对比
“颜色混合”任务数据来源 | 得分情况 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 | 1 | 0 | |||||
实验2b | 36月以下组(n = 11) | 11 (45.83%) | 5 (45.45%) | 5 (20.83%) | 2 (18.18%) | 8 (33.33%) | 4 (36.36%) |
36月及以上组(n = 13) | 6 (46.15%) | 3 (23.08%) | 4 (30.77%) | ||||
Moll & | 15 (62.50%) | 8 (33.33%) | 1 (4.17%) |
参考文献 50
1 | A?te A., Berthoz A., Vidal J., Ro?ll M., Zaoui M., Houdé O., & Borst G . ( 2016). Taking a third-person perspective requires inhibitory control: Evidence from a developmental negative priming study. Child Development, 87(6), 1825-1840. |
2 | , Apperly I.A., &Butterfill S.A, . ( 2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953-970. |
3 | Barragan R.C., & Dweck C.S . ( 2014). Rethinking natural altruism: Simple reciprocal interactions trigger children’s benevolence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(48), 17071-17074. |
4 | Bukowski H., Hietanen J. K., & Samson D . ( 2016). From gaze cueing to perspective taking: Revisiting the claim that we automatically compute where or what other people are looking at. Visual Cognition, 23(8), 1020-1042. |
5 | Carpendale J. I.M., &Lewis C. , ( 2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(1), 79-96. |
6 | , Carpendale J. I.M., &Lewis C. , ( 2015). The development of social understanding.Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. |
7 | Converse B. A., Lin S., Keysar B., & Epley N . ( 2008). In the mood to get over yourself: mood affects theory-of-mind use.Emotion, 8(5), 725-730. |
8 | Epley N., Keysar B., van Boven L., & Gilovich T . ( 2004). Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(3), 327-339. 3. 327 |
9 | Flavell J.H . ( 1974). The development of inferences about others. In Understanding other persons.(p. xv, 266-xv, 266). Oxford, England: Rowman and Littlefield. |
10 | Flavell J. H., Everett B. A., Croft K., & Flavell E. R . ( 1981). Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1-Level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 99-103. . 1. 99 |
11 | Freundlieb M., Sebanz N., & Kovács á. M . ( 2017). Out of your sight, out of my mind: Knowledge about another person’s visual access modulates spontaneous visuospatial perspective-taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology?: Human Perception and Performance, 43(6), 1065-1072. |
12 | Frick A., M?hring W., & Newcombe N. S . ( 2014). Picturing perspectives: Development of perspective-taking abilities in 4- to 8-year-olds. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 386. |
13 | Gopnik A., &Astington J.W . ( 1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction.Child Development, 59(1), 26-37. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb03192. x |
14 | Hamlin J. K., Mahajan N., Liberman Z., & Wynn K . ( 2013). Not like me = bad: Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others.Psychological Science, 24(4), 589-594. |
15 | Hou X., Gong Z., Yan Z., Su Y., & Zuo X . ( 2018). Children’s theory of mind development: Cultural perspectives. Chinese Science Bulletin,( 4), 384-392. |
16 | [ 侯晓晖, 宫竹青, 颜志雄, 苏彦捷, 左西年 . ( 2018). 儿童心理理论发生与发展: 跨文化的视角. 科学通报,(4),384-392.] |
17 | Jin X., Li P., He J., & Shen M . ( 2017). Cooperation, but not competition, improves 4-year-old children’s reasoning about others’ diverse desires. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 157, 81-94. . 2016. 12. 010 |
18 | Jin X., Li P., He J., & Shen M . ( 2018). How you act matters: The impact of coordination on 4-year-old children’s reasoning about diverse desires. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 176,13-25. |
19 | Johnson D.W . ( 1975). Cooperativeness and social perspective taking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(2), 241-244. |
20 | Lang P. J., Bradley M. M., & Cuthbert B. N . ( 1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention. |
21 | Lanzetta, J. T., & Englis, B. G . ( 1989). Expectations of cooperation and competition and their effects on observers’ vicarious emotional responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(4), 543-554. |
22 | Lewis C., Freeman N. H., Kyriakidou C., Maridaki- Kassotaki K., & Berridge D. M . ( 1996). Social influences on false belief access: Specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Development, 67(6), 2930-2947. |
23 | Li P., Jin X., Liao Y., Li Y., Shen M., & He J . ( 2019). Cooperation turns preschoolers into flexible perspective takers. Manuscript in preparation. |
24 | Lin B., Cheng L., Li Q., & Wu C . ( 2003). A developmental intervention study on children’s social perspective taking. Psychological science.26( 6), 1030-1033. |
25 | [ 林彬, 程利国, 李其维, 吴昌旭 . ( 2003). 儿童社会观点采择能力发展的干预研究. 心理科学.26( 6), 1030-1033.] |
26 | Liu G. ( 2014). Intervening measures of children’s perspective taking and revelation on education of autism. Journal of Mudanjiang College of Education, ( 3), 48-50. |
27 | [ 刘桂宏 . ( 2014). 儿童观点采择的干预措施及对自闭症儿童教育的启示. 牡丹江教育学院学报.( 3), 48-50.] |
28 | Marcovitch S., O’Brien M., Calkins S. D., Leerkes E. M., Weaver J. M., & Levine D. W . ( 2015). A longitudinal assessment of the relation between executive function and theory of mind at 3, 4, and 5 years.Cognitive Development. |
29 | Masangkay Z. S., McCluskey K. A., McIntyre C. W., Sims-Knight J., Vaughn B. E., & Flavell J. H . ( 1974). The early development of inferences about the visual percepts of others.Child Development, 45(2), 357-366. |
30 | Mattan B. D., Rotshtein P., & Quinn K. A . ( 2016). Empathy and visual perspective-taking performance.Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1-4), 170-181. |
31 | Meltzoff A.N . ( 2007). ‘Like me’: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10(1), 126-134. |
32 | Michael J., Sebanz N., & Knoblich G . ( 2016). Observing joint action: Coordination creates commitment. Cognition, 157,106-113. |
33 | Moll H., &Meltzoff A.N . ( 2011). How does it look? Level 2 perspective‐taking at 36 months of age. Child Development, 82(2), 661-673. . x |
34 | Moll H.., &Tomasello M. , ( 2007). Cooperation and human cognition: The Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.Series B, Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 639-648. |
35 | Piaget J. ( 1995). Sociological studies.Routledge. |
36 | Piaget J. &Inhelder B. , ( 1956). The child’s concept of space. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. |
37 | Qureshi A. W., Apperly I. A., & Samson D . ( 2010). Executive function is necessary for perspective selection, not Level-1 visual perspective calculation: Evidence from a dual-task study of adults .Cognition, 117(2), 230-236. |
38 | SigirtmacA .D . ( 2016). An investigation on the effectiveness of chess training on creativity and theory of mind development at early childhood . Educational Research and Reviews, 11(11), 1056-1063. > |
39 | Simpson A.J., & Todd A.R, . ( 2017). Intergroup visual perspective-taking: Shared group membership impairs self-perspective inhibition but may facilitate perspective calculation. Cognition, 166, 371-381. |
40 | Surtees A., Apperly I., & Samson D . ( 2016). I’ve got your number: Spontaneous perspective-taking in an interactive task. Cognition, 150, 43-52. 2016. 01. 014 |
41 | Todd A.R., & Simpson A.J, . ( 2016). Anxiety impairs spontaneous perspective calculation: Evidence from a level-1 visual perspective-taking task. Cognition, 156, 88-94. . 2016. 08. 004 |
42 | Tomasello M., & Carpenter M. , ( 2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121-125. . x |
43 | Vygotsky, L. S . ( 1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Mind in Society The Development of Higher Psychological Processes,Mind in So, 159. |
44 | Warneken F., Chen F., & Tomasello M . ( 2006). Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development, 7,(3), 640-663. . x |
45 | Wu S., Barr D. J., Gann T. M., & Keysar B . ( 2013). How culture influences perspective taking: differences in correction, not integration. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 822. |
46 | Yin J., Xu H., Ding X., Liang J., Shui R., & Shen M . ( 2016). Social constraints from an observer’s perspective: Coordinated actions make an agent’s position more predictable. Cognition, 151,10-17. 2016. 02. 009 |
47 | , Zhang W.X., &Lin C.D, . ( 1999). The development of children's social perspective-taking and its relation to their peer interactions. Acta Psychologica Sinica.31( 4), 418-427. |
48 | [ 张文新, 林崇德 . ( 1999). 儿童社会观点采择的发展及其与同伴互动关系的研究. 心理学报.31( 4), 418-427.] |
49 | Zhao J., Wang L., & Su Y-J . ( 2010). Origin development of visual perspective-taking and its relations with some other abilities. Psychological Development and Education.26( 1), 107-111. |
50 | [ 赵婧, 王璐, 苏彦捷 . ( 2010). 视觉观点采择的发生发展及其影响因素. 心理发展与教育.26( 1), 107-111.] |
相关文章 15
[1] | 熊承清, 许佳颖, 马丹阳, 刘永芳. 囚徒困境博弈中对手面部表情对合作行为的影响及其作用机制[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 919-932. |
[2] | 陈思静, 邢懿琳, 翁异静, 黎常. 第三方惩罚对合作的溢出效应:基于社会规范的解释[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(7): 758-772. |
[3] | 苗晓燕, 孙欣, 匡仪, 汪祚军. 共患难, 更同盟:共同经历相同负性情绪事件促进合作行为[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(1): 81-94. |
[4] | 张丹丹, 王驹, 赵君, 陈淑美, 黄琰淋, 高秋凤. 抑郁倾向对合作的影响:双人同步近红外脑成像研究[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(5): 609-622. |
[5] | 姚尧, 陈晓湘. 音乐训练对4~5岁幼儿普通话声调范畴感知能力的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(4): 456-468. |
[6] | 贺晓玲, 陈俊. 3~5岁幼儿权力概念多重隐喻的认知发展[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(2): 149-161. |
[7] | 王建峰, 戴冰. “追名弃利”:权力动机与社会存在对亲社会行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 55-65. |
[8] | 李泉, 宋亚男, 廉彬, 冯廷勇. 正念训练提升3~4岁幼儿注意力和执行功能[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(3): 324-336. |
[9] | 唐卫海, 钟汝波, 许晓旭, 刘希平. 面孔吸引力和信息正确性对幼儿选择性信任的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(1): 71-84. |
[10] | 李婷玉, 刘黎, 李宜霖, 朱莉琪. 冲突情境下幼儿的选择性信任和信念修正[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(12): 1390-1399. |
[11] | 窦凯, 刘耀中, 王玉洁, 聂衍刚. “乐”于合作:感知社会善念诱导合作行为的情绪机制[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(1): 101-114. |
[12] | 柯淳淳, 聂爱情, 张瑞卿. 回忆任务对合作抑制和错误修剪的调节 ——情绪效价和编码水平的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(6): 733-744. |
[13] | 崔丽莹, 何幸, 罗俊龙, 黄晓娇, 曹玮佳, 陈晓梅. 道德与关系惩罚对初中生公共物品困境中合作行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(10): 1322-1333. |
[14] | 李婷玉;刘黎;朱莉琪. 4~6岁幼儿经济博弈中的信任行为及其影响因素[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(1): 17-27. |
[15] | 胡清芬;卢静. 幼儿在地图任务中对自我位置与自我朝向的结合[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(9): 1143-1150. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4513