删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

冲突情境下幼儿的选择性信任和信念修正

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

李婷玉1,2, 刘黎1,2, 李宜霖1,2, 朱莉琪1()
1 中国科学院行为科学重点实验室(中国科学院心理研究所), 北京 100101
2 中国科学院大学, 北京 100049
收稿日期:2018-01-02出版日期:2018-11-30发布日期:2018-10-30


基金资助:* 国家社会科学基金重大项目(14ZDB161)支持

Preschoolers' selective trust and belief revision in conflicting situation

LI Tingyu1,2, LIU Li1,2, LI Yilin1,2, ZHU Liqi1()
1 CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Beijing 100101, China
2 University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
Received:2018-01-02Online:2018-11-30Published:2018-10-30







摘要/Abstract


摘要: 幼儿如何选择性地采信他人提供的信息、获取知识, 是幼儿社会认知发展研究的重要问题。除客观的信息内容外, 信息传递者的特征也影响幼儿的选择性信任。另外, 在不确定情境下, 他人证言与幼儿已有信念可能存在冲突, 在二者冲突的情境下, 幼儿是否可以采信他人的证言, 并最终改变幼儿的已有判断是以往研究忽略的问题。本研究考察了当母亲证言与幼儿已有信念冲突时, 不同程度的冲突情境(面对不同比例的合成动物时)对幼儿选择性信任和信念修正的影响。研究采用冲突信息源范式, 向74名4~6岁幼儿呈现对不同比例合成动物(50%-50%任务 vs 75%-25%任务)命名的证言, 考察幼儿对信息提供者的询问意愿、对证言的信任情况和在证言前后对合成动物的判断(信念修正)。信息提供的一方为母亲(提供相似度低的动物名称), 另一方为陌生人(提供相似度高的动物名称)。结果发现, 幼儿的选择性信任和信念修正受到母亲证言与已有信念冲突程度的影响。在低冲突情境下, 幼儿对母亲证言的询问意愿和外显信任均高于高冲突情境。幼儿的信念修正存在年龄差异, 在低冲突情境下, 年长幼儿比年幼幼儿更倾向于改变已有信念, 在高冲突情境下, 年长的幼儿比年幼幼儿更不愿意改变已有信念。本研究结果表明, 当母亲的证言与幼儿已有信念冲突时, 中国幼儿可以区分不同程度的冲突情境, 并且可以根据冲突的程度进行选择性信任和信念修正。



图150%-50%合成动物(左为牛-马, 右为熊-猪)
图150%-50%合成动物(左为牛-马, 右为熊-猪)



图225%-75%合成动物(左为鱼-鸟, 右为松鼠-兔子)
图225%-75%合成动物(左为鱼-鸟, 右为松鼠-兔子)


表1不同合成动物任务中的实验材料与不同信息提供者的证言
任务 实验材料 母亲 陌生人
50%-50%合成动物 牛-马
熊-猪
25%-75%合成动物 鱼-鸟
松鼠-兔子 松鼠 兔子

表1不同合成动物任务中的实验材料与不同信息提供者的证言
任务 实验材料 母亲 陌生人
50%-50%合成动物 牛-马
熊-猪
25%-75%合成动物 鱼-鸟
松鼠-兔子 松鼠 兔子


表2幼儿选择询问母亲的比率(年龄组内%)
组别 50%-50%合成动物 25%-75%合成动物
4岁组 65.10 69.85
5岁组 63.95 63.90
6岁组 88.90** 88.10**

表2幼儿选择询问母亲的比率(年龄组内%)
组别 50%-50%合成动物 25%-75%合成动物
4岁组 65.10 69.85
5岁组 63.95 63.90
6岁组 88.90** 88.10**


表3幼儿选择信任母亲证言的比率(年龄组内%)
组别 50%-50%合成动物 25%-75%合成动物
4岁组 56.35 14.30**
5岁组 53.30 8.20**
6岁组 67.45 9.55**

表3幼儿选择信任母亲证言的比率(年龄组内%)
组别 50%-50%合成动物 25%-75%合成动物
4岁组 56.35 14.30**
5岁组 53.30 8.20**
6岁组 67.45 9.55**


表4幼儿初始和最终判断与母亲证言一致的比率(年龄组内%)
组别 50%-50%合成动物 25%-75%合成动物
初始 最终 初始 最终
4岁组 12.70** 32.55 1.60** 13.50**
5岁组 21.45* 34.45 1.65** 9.05**
6岁组 12.70** 32.55 0.00** 9.55**

表4幼儿初始和最终判断与母亲证言一致的比率(年龄组内%)
组别 50%-50%合成动物 25%-75%合成动物
初始 最终 初始 最终
4岁组 12.70** 32.55 1.60** 13.50**
5岁组 21.45* 34.45 1.65** 9.05**
6岁组 12.70** 32.55 0.00** 9.55**







1 Akhtar N., &Jaswal , V. K . ( 2013). Deficit or difference? Interpreting diverse developmental paths: An introduction to the special section. Developmental Psychology, 49( 1), 1-3.
doi: 10.1037/a0029851URLpmid: 23316772
2 Alchourrón C. E., G?rdenfors P., & Makinson D. C . ( 2014). On the logic of theory change: Partial meet contraction and revision functions. Journal of Symbolic Logic, 50( 2), 510-530.
3 Amsterlaw J., &Wellman , H. M . ( 2006). Theories of mind in transition: A microgenetic study of the development of false belief understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7( 2), 139-172.
doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_1URL
4 Bascandziev I., &Harris P.L, . ( 2011). Gravity is not the only ruler for falling events: Young children stop making the gravity error after receiving additional perceptual information about the tubes mechanism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109( 4), 468-477.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.010URLpmid: 21497828
5 Bascandziev I., &Harris P.L, . ( 2014). In beauty we trust: children prefer information from more attractive informants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32( 1), 94-99.
doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12022URLpmid: 24164592
6 Birch S. A. J., Akmal N., & Frampton K. L . ( 2010). Two-year-olds are vigilant of others’ non-verbal cues to credibility. Developmental Science, 13( 2), 363-369.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00906.xURLpmid: 20136933
7 Boseovski J.J., &Thurman S.L, . ( 2014). Evaluating and approaching a strange animal: Children's trust in informant testimony. Child Development, 85( 2), 824-834.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12156URLpmid: 24032359
8 Carey S.., ( 1991). Knowledge acquisition: enrichment or conceptual change? In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 257-291). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
doi: 10.1097/00006324-196809000-00003URL
9 Chan, C. C. Y., &Tardif , T. ( 2013). Knowing better: The role of prior knowledge and culture in trust in testimony. Developmental Psychology, 49( 3), 591-601.
doi: 10.1037/a0031336URLpmid: 23294151
10 Chen X. Y., Rubin K. H., Liu M. W., Chen H. C., Wang L., Li D., .. Li B. S . ( 2003). Compliance in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27( 5), 428-436.
doi: 10.1080/01650250344000046URL
11 Chouinard M. M., Harris P. L., & Maratsos M. P . ( 2007). Children's questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72( 1), Serial No. 286, i-129.
doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2007.00412.xURLpmid: 17394580
12 Clément F., Koenig M., & Harris P . ( 2004). The ontogenesis of trust. Mind & Language, 19 (4), 360-379.
13 Coady C. AJ.. ( 1992) . Testimony: A philosophical study Oxford: Clarendon Press A philosophical study. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
14 Corriveau K., &Harris P.L, . ( 2009). Choosing your informant: Weighing familiarity and recent accuracy. Developmental Science, 12( 3), 426-437.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00792.xURLpmid: 19371367
15 , Corriveau K.H., &Harris P.L, . ( 2010). Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. Developmental Psychology, 46( 2), 437-445.
doi: 10.1037/a0017553URLpmid: 20210502
16 Corriveau K. H., Harris P. L., Meins E., Fernyhough C., Arnott B., Elliott L., .. de Rosnay M . ( 2009). Young children’s trust in their mother’s claims: Longitudinal links with attachment security in infancy. Child Development, 80( 3), 750-761.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01295.xURLpmid: 1948990119489901
17 de Leeuw N. , ( 1993, June). Students’ beliefs about the circulatory system: are misconceptions universal? In Proceedings of the Fifteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 389-393), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
18 Doan T., Denison S., Lucas C., & Gopnik A . ( 2015). Learning to reason about desires: An infant training study. In D. C. Noelle et al. (Eds.), Proceedings for the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 578-583). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
19 Einav S., &Robinson , E. J . ( 2010). Children's sensitivity to error magnitude when evaluating informants. Cognitive Development, 25( 3), 218-232.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.04.002URL
20 Ganea P. A., Shutts K., Spelke E. S., & DeLoache J. S . ( 2007). Thinking of things unseen: Infants' use of language to update mental representations. Psychological Science, 18( 8), 734-739.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01968.xURLpmid: 17680946
21 Ho D. Y. F.( 1986) . Chinese patterns of socialization: a critical review In M H Bond (Ed), The psychology of the Chinese people (pp 1-37) New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press a critical review. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people. (pp. 1-37). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
22 Jaswal V.K . ( 2004). Don't believe everything you hear: Preschoolers' sensitivity to speaker intent in category induction. Child Development, 75( 6), 1871-1885.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00822.xURLpmid: 15566385
23 Jaswal V.K . ( 2006). Preschoolers favor the creator's label when reasoning about an artifact's function. Cognition, 99( 3), B83-B92.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.07.006URLpmid: 16185679
24 Jaswal V.K., &Malone , L. S . ( 2007). Turning believers into skeptics: 3-year-olds' sensitivity to cues to speaker credibility. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8( 3), 263-283.
doi: 10.1080/15248370701446392URL
25 Jaswal V.K., &Markman , E. M . ( 2007). Looks aren't everything: 24-month-olds' willingness to accept unexpected labels. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8( 1), 93-111.
26 Jaswal V. K., Pérez-Edgar K., Kondrad RL., Palmquist C. M., Cole C. A., & Cole C. E . ( 2014). Can't stop believing: Inhibitory control and resistance to misleading testimony. Developmental Science, 17( 6), 965-976.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12187URLpmid: 24806881
27 Kinzler K. D., Corriveau K. H., & Harris P. L . ( 2011). Children's selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science, 14( 1), 106-111.
28 Koenig M.A., &Echols , C. H . ( 2003). Infants' understanding of false labeling events: The referential roles of words and the speakers who use them. Cognition, 87( 3), 179-208.
doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00002-7URLpmid: 12684199
29 Kushnir T., &Gopnik A. , ( 2007). Conditional probability versus spatial contiguity in causal learning: Preschoolers use new contingency evidence to overcome prior spatial assumptions. Developmental Psychology, 43( 1), 186-196.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.186URLpmid: 17201518
30 Lane J. D., Harris P. L., Gelman S. A., & Wellman H. M . ( 2014). More than meets the eye: Young children’s trust in claims that defy their perceptions. Developmental Psychology, 50( 3), 865-871.
doi: 10.1037/a0034291
31 Lucas A. J., Lewis C., Pala F. C., Wong K., & Berridge D . ( 2013). Social-cognitive processes in preschoolers' selective trust: Three cultures compared. Developmental Psychology, 49( 3), 579-590.
doi: 10.1037/a0029864URLpmid: 22946437
32 Lucas C. G., Bridgers S., Griffiths T. L., & Gopnik A . ( 2014). When children are better (or at least more open-minded) learners than adults: Developmental differences in learning the forms of causal relationships. Cognition, 131( 2), 284-299.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.010URLpmid: 24566007
33 Luo G.., (1996). Chinese traditional social and moral ideas and rules. Beijing, China: The University of Chinese People Press.
34 Melnyk L., Crossman A. M., & Scullin M. H . ( 2007). The suggestibility of children's memory. In M. P. Toglia, J. D. Read, D. F. Ross, & R. C. L. Lindsay (Eds.), The handbook of eyewitness psychology, Vol I: Memory for events. (pp. 401-427). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
35 Robinson E. J., Mitchell P., & Nye R . ( 1995). Young children's treating of utterances as unreliable sources of knowledge. Journal of Child Language, 22( 3), 663-685.
doi: 10.1017/S0305000900009995URLpmid: 8789518
36 Schauble L ( 1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49( 1), 31-57.
doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(90)90048-DURLpmid: 2303776
37 Slaughter V., &Gopnik A. , ( 1996). Conceptual coherence in the child's theory of mind: Training children to understand belief. Child Development, 67( 6), 2967-2988.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01898.xURLpmid: 9071768
38 Vosniadou S. &Verschaffel L. , ( 2004). Extending the conceptual change approach to mathematics learning and teaching. Learning and Instruction, 14( 5), 445-451.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.014URL
39 Young A. G., Alibali M. W., & Kalish C. W . ( 2012). Disagreement and causal learning: Others' hypotheses affect children's evaluations of evidence. Developmental Psychology, 48(5), 1242-1253.
doi: 10.1037/a0027540URLpmid: 22390663
40 , , Zarbatany L. &Lamb M.E, . ( 1985). Social referencing as a function of information source: Mothers versus strangers. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(1), 25-33.
41 , Zhang Y.H., &Zhu L.Q, . ( 2014). Epistemic trust: How preschoolers selectively learn from others. Advances in Psychological Science,22( 1), 86-96.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00086URL
42 [ 张耀华, 朱莉琪 . ( 2014). 认识性信任: 学龄前儿童的选择性学习. 心理科学进展, 22( 1), 86-96.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00086URL




[1]周文洁, 邓丽群, 丁锦红. 物体颜色对情景记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(3): 229-243.
[2]刘贵雄,贾永萍,王余娟,买合甫来提·坎吉,郭春彦. 联结再认中双语者第二语言记忆优势效应[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(1): 14-23.
[3]唐卫海,钟汝波,许晓旭,刘希平. 面孔吸引力和信息正确性对幼儿选择性信任的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(1): 71-84.
[4]李婷玉;刘黎;朱莉琪. 4~6岁幼儿经济博弈中的信任行为及其影响因素[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(1): 17-27.
[5]贾永萍;周楚;李林;郭秀艳. 汉字的无线索回忆再认效应:重复学习和重复测验的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(2): 111-120.
[6]叶晓红;陈幼贞;孟迎芳. 回想、熟悉性与启动在编码过程的认知神经机制[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(9): 1101-1110.
[7]毛新瑞;徐慧芳;郭春彦. 双加工再认提取中的情绪记忆增强效应[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(9): 1111-1123.
[8]陈莉;王沛;谢轶雯;张琴. 熟悉性和兼容性对复合社会范畴刻板印象表征的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(3): 375-388.
[9]陈永香;朱莉琪. 影响动作图片命名的因素[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(1): 11-18.
[10]张积家,董昌锋. 范畴变量对虚假记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2006, 38(03): 324-332.
[11]李寿欣,丁兆叶,张利增. 认知方式与线索特征对前瞻记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2005, 37(03): 320-327.
[12]韩凯,沈大为,李波. FOK产生机制的再验证[J]. 心理学报, 2001, 33(1): 13-16.
[13]罗劲,林仲贤. FOK的线索熟悉性假说与线索熟悉性判断[J]. 心理学报, 1999, 31(2): 121-127.
[14]罗劲,林仲贤. 对FOK的线索熟悉性假说的重新检验[J]. 心理学报, 1998, 30(3): 241-247.
[15]韩松梅,朱滢. 再认的两种机制及其与启动效应的关系[J]. 心理学报, 1995, 27(1): 9-14.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4325
相关话题/动物 心理 信息 材料 实验