删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

文化演化的认知视角——从个体社会学习出发探究文化动态性

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

陈维扬1, 谢天2()
1西南财经大学社会发展研究院, 成都 611130
2武汉大学哲学学院心理学系, 武汉 430072
收稿日期:2020-04-01出版日期:2020-12-15发布日期:2020-10-26
通讯作者:谢天E-mail:thanksky520@126.com

基金资助:中央高校基本科研业务费专项资金资助(社会学习视角下文化进化的认知机制研究)(JBK200510)

The cognitive perspective of cultural evolution: Exploring cultural dynamics from the view of social learning

CHEN Weiyang1, XIE Tian2()
1Research Institute of Social Development, Southwestern University of Finance and Economics, Chengdu 611130, China
2Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Wuhan University, Wuhan 430072, China
Received:2020-04-01Online:2020-12-15Published:2020-10-26
Contact:XIE Tian E-mail:thanksky520@126.com






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 文化演化是多学科共同关心的文化研究主题。文化演化的认知视角是心理学家切入该研究主题的一种方式, 它着眼于社会文化环境中的个体认知, 研究个体在社会学习过程中对文化信息的加工、改变、记忆与提取。文化演化的认知视角借用达尔文生物演化理论, 涉及文化传承、创新、选择三个子领域, 提出了文化演化的三原则: 遗传、变异、选择。文化传承的路径包括模仿和教导, 类型分为工具性与习俗性文化传承; 文化创新具有层次性, 人类特有的累积性文化演化建立在文化创新的基础之上, 体现了文化创新的代际传递; 基于行为生态学和人类认知机制的文化选择造成了文化信息的差异化适应。未来研究可以从研究概念、研究思路、研究方法三方面推进认知视角下的文化演化研究, 探索更高层次的文化创新, 扩展文化演化的前因变量, 结合新技术加深对文化演化的理解; 发挥文化演化对文化心理学研究的助推作用, 研究文化混搭、个性心理特征对文化演化的影响。


[1] 陈维扬, 谢天. (2018). 社会规范的动态过程. 心理科学进展, 26(7), 1284-1293.
[2] 丰怡, 蔡华俭, 施媛媛. (2013). 文化产品研究——文化心理学的独特视角. 心理科学进展, 21(2), 326-335.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00326URL
[3] 黄兴涛. (2019). “新文化”的出版姿态、知识构建与社会传播——1923年商务版《新文化辞书》的历史透视. 中国人民大学学报, (6), 158-169.
[4] 彭璐珞, 郑晓莹, 彭泗清. (2017). 文化混搭: 研究现状与发展方向. 心理科学进展, 25(7), 1240-1250.
[5] 辛自强, 刘国芳. (2012). 文化进化的实验与非实验研究方法. 北京师范大学学报: 社会科学版, (3), 5-13.
[6] 俞国良, 谢天. (2014). 文化变迁研究的进展与前瞻. 黑龙江社会科学, (4), 91-96.
[7] 赵志裕, 吴莹, 杨宜音. (2015). 文化混搭: 文化与心理研究的新里程. 中国社会心理学评论, 9, 1-18.
[8] 郑路. (2017). 大数据为社会网络分析带来“黄金时代”. 实证社会科学, 4, 78-86.
[9] 邹小燕, 尹可丽, 陆林. (2018). 集体仪式促进凝聚力: 基于动作, 情绪与记忆. 心理科学进展, 26(5), 939-950.
[10] Acerbi, A., & Tennie, C. (2016). The role of redundant information in cultural transmission and cultural stabilization. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(1), 62-70.
doi: 10.1037/a0040094URLpmid: 26881945
[11] Amato, R., Lacasa, L., Díaz-Guilera, A., & Baronchelli, A. (2018). The dynamics of norm change in the cultural evolution of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(33), 8260-8265.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721059115URL
[12] Broesch,T., & Bryant,G. A. (2018). Fathers' infant-directed speech in a small-scale society. Child Development, 89(2), e29-e41.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.12768URLpmid: 28239835
[13] Caldwell, C. A., Atkinson, M., & Renner, E. (2016). Experimental approaches to studying cumulative cultural evolution. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(3), 191-195.
doi: 10.1177/0963721416641049URLpmid: 27397972
[14] Caldwell, C. A., Renner, E., & Atkinson, M. (2018). Human teaching and cumulative cultural evolution. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 9(4), 751-770.
doi: 10.1007/s13164-017-0346-3URLpmid: 30595765
[15] Chiu, C. -Y., Gries, P. H.., Torelli, C. J., & Cheng, S. Y. Y. (2011). Toward a social psychology of globalization. Journal of Social Issues, 67(4), 663-676.
doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2011.01721.xURL
[16] Chudek, M., Heller, S., Birch, S., & Henrich, J. (2012). Prestige-biased cultural learning: Bystander's differential attention to potential models influences children's learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(1), 46-56.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.05.005URL
[17] Clegg, J. M., & Legare,C. H. (2016). A cross-cultural comparison of children’s imitative flexibility. Developmental Psychology, 52(9), 1435-1444.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000131URLpmid: 27570982
[18] Clegg, J. M., & Legare,C. H. (2017). Parents scaffold flexible imitation during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 153, 1-14.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.004URLpmid: 27676182
[19] Collard, M., Ruttle, A., Buchanan, B., & O’Brien, M. J. (2013). Population size and cultural evolution in nonindustrial food-producing societies. Plos ONE, 8(9). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0072628
URLpmid: 24116258
[20] Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2013). Evolutionary psychology: New perspectives on cognition and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 201-229.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131628URL
[21] Dean, L. G., Vale, G. L., Laland, K. N., Flynn, E. G., & Kendal, R. L. (2014). Human cumulative culture: A comparative perspective. Biological Reviews, 89(2), 284-301.
doi: 10.1111/brv.12053URL
[22] Derex, M., Godelle, B., & Raymond, M. (2014). How does competition affect the transmission of information? Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(2), 89-95.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.11.001URL
[23] Fabry, R. E. (2017). Cognitive innovation, cumulative cultural evolution, and enculturation. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 17(5), 375-395.
doi: 10.1163/15685373-12340014URL
[24] Fay, N., de Kleine, N., Walker, B., & Caldwell, C. A. (2019). Increasing population size can inhibit cumulative cultural evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(14), 6726-6731.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811413116URL
[25] Fay,N., & Ellison,T. M. (2013). The cultural evolution of human communication systems in different sized populations: Usability trumps learnability. Plos ONE, 8(8). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071781
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072387URLpmid: 24058411
[26] Fay, N., Ellison, T. M., Tylén, K., Fusaroli, R., Walker, B., & Garrod, S. (2018). Applying the cultural ratchet to a social artefact: The cumulative cultural evolution of a language game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(3), 300-309.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.02.002URL
[27] Fessler, D. M., Pisor, A. C., & Navarrete, C. D. (2014). Negatively-biased credulity and the cultural evolution of beliefs. Plos ONE, 9(4). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095167
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096151URLpmid: 24788601
[28] Greenfield, P. M. (2013). The changing psychology of culture from 1800 through 2000. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1722-1731.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613479387URLpmid: 23925305
[29] Hamamura, T., & Xu, Y. (2015). Changes in Chinese culture as examined through changes in personal pronoun usage. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(7), 930-941.
doi: 10.1177/0022022115592968URL
[30] Heine, S. J. (2015). Cultural psychology: Third international student edition. London: W.W. Norton & company.
[31] Heyes, C. (2018). Enquire within: Cultural evolution and cognitive science. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1743). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0051
[32] Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews, 69(2), 207-231.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185x.1994.tb01506.xURLpmid: 8054445
[33] Kashima, Y., Bain, P. G., & Perfors, A. (2019). The psychology of cultural dynamics: What is it, what do we know, and what is yet to be known? Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 499-529.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103112URL
[34] Kennedy, D., & Norman, C. (2005). What don't we know? Science, 309(5731), 78-102.
doi: 10.1126/science.309.5731.78aURLpmid: 15994523
[35] Kline, M. A., Shamsudheen, R., & Broesch, T. (2018). Variation is the universal: Making cultural evolution work in developmental psychology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1743). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0059
[36] Kumaraswamy, A., Garud, R., & Ansari, S. (2018). Perspectives on disruptive innovations. Journal of Management Studies, 55(7), 1025-1042.
doi: 10.1111/joms.v55.7URL
[37] Lancy, D. F. (2014). The anthropology of childhood: Cherubs, chattel, changelings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[38] Legare, C. H. (2017). Cumulative cultural learning: Development and diversity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7877-7883.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620743114URL
[39] Legare, C. H., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(11), 688-699.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.005URLpmid: 26440121
[40] Legare, C. H., Wen, N. J., Herrmann, P. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2015). Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning. Cognition, 142, 351-361.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.020URLpmid: 26083314
[41] Leite, S. F. (2016). Successes in cultural evolution raises the variability in humans’ highest stage attained. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 21(2), 165-175.
doi: 10.1037/bdb0000033URL
[42] Lindenfors, P., Envall, I., Isaksson, S., & Enquist, M. (2015). An empirical study of cultural evolution: The development of European cookery from medieval to modern times. Cliodynamics: The Journal of Theoretical & Mathematical History, 6(2), 115-129.
[43] McElreath, R., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (2003). Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology, 44(1), 122-130.
doi: 10.1086/345689URL
[44] Mesoudi, A. (2007). Using the methods of experimental social psychology to study cultural evolution. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 1(2), 35-58.
doi: 10.1037/h0099359URL
[45] Mesoudi, A. (2016). Cultural evolution: A review of theory, findings and controversies. Evolutionary Biology, 43(4), 481-497.
doi: 10.1007/s11692-015-9320-0URL
[46] Mesoudi, A. (2017). Pursuing Darwin’s curious parallel: Prospects for a science of cultural evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7853-7860.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620741114URL
[47] Mesoudi, A., Magid, K., & Hussain, D. (2016). How do people become W.E.I.R.D.? Migration reveals the cultural transmission mechanisms underlying variation in psychological processes. Plos ONE, 11(1). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147162
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147434URLpmid: 26824864
[48] Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Dunbar, R. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology, 97(3), 405-423.
doi: 10.1348/000712605X85871URL
[49] Molleman, L., van den Berg, P., & Weissing, F. J. (2014). Consistent individual differences in human social learning strategies. Nature Communications, 5(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms4570
[50] Morris, M. W., Chiu, C. -Y., & Liu, Z. (2015). Polycultural psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 631-659.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015001URLpmid: 25251481
[51] Muthukrishna, M., Morgan, T. J. H., & Henrich, J. (2016). The when and who of social learning and conformist transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(1), 10-20.
doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.05.004URL
[52] Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada,J. N. S. (2008). Adaptive memory: Is survival processing special? Journal of Memory and Language, 59(3), 377-385.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.001URL
[53] Nakahashi, W., Wakano, J. Y., & Henrich, J. (2012). Adaptive social learning strategies in temporally and spatially varying environments. Human Nature, 23(4), 386-418.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-012-9151-yURLpmid: 22926986
[54] Nettle, D. (2019). State-dependent cognition and its relevance to cultural evolution. Behavioural Processes, 161, 101-107.
doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.01.018URLpmid: 29421223
[55] Nielsen, M., Moore, C., & Mohamedally, J. (2012). Young children overimitate in third-party contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(1), 73-83.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.001URL
[56] Qiu, L., Chan, S. H. M., & Chan, D. (2018). Big data in social and psychological science: Theoretical and methodological issues. Journal of Computational Social Science, 1(1), 59-66.
doi: 10.1007/s42001-017-0013-6URL
[57] Rawlings, B., Flynn, E., & Kendal, R. (2017). To copy or to innovate? The role of personality and social networks in children's learning strategies. Child Development Perspectives, 11(1), 39-44.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.12206URL
[58] Schulz, J. F., Bahramirad-Rad, D., Beauchamp, J. P., & Henrich, J. (2019). The Church, intensive kinship, and global psychological variation. Science, 366(6466). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aau5141
doi: 10.1126/science.aaz4859URLpmid: 31699921
[59] Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2017). A (simple) experimental demonstration that cultural evolution is not replicative, but reconstructive - and an explanation of why this difference matters. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 17(1-2), 1-11.
doi: 10.1163/15685373-12342188URL
[60] Slater, M., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2016). Enhancing our lives with immersive virtual reality. Frontiers in Robotics and AI, 30. https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2016.00074
[61] Sparrow, B., Liu, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2011). Google effects on memory: Cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science, 333(6043), 776-778.
doi: 10.1126/science.1206773URL
[62] Stout,D., & Hecht,E. E. (2017). Evolutionary neuroscience of cumulative culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7861-7868.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620738114URL
[63] Stubbersfield, J. M., Tehrani, J. J., & Flynn, E. G. (2015). Serial killers, spiders and cybersex: Social and survival information bias in the transmission of urban legends. British Journal of Psychology, 106(2), 288-307.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12073URLpmid: 24975479
[64] Tam, K. -P. (2015). Understanding intergenerational cultural transmission through the role of perceived norms. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(10), 1260-1266.
doi: 10.1177/0022022115600074URL
[65] Varnum, M. E. W., & Grossmann, I. (2017). Cultural change: The how and the why. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 956-972.
doi: 10.1177/1745691617699971URLpmid: 28915361
[66] Wasielewski, H. (2014). Imitation is necessary for cumulative cultural evolution in an unfamiliar, opaque task. Human Nature, 25(1), 161-179.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-014-9192-5URLpmid: 24519404
[67] Watson-Jones, R. E., Whitehouse, H., & Legare, C. H. (2016). In-group ostracism increases high-fidelity imitation in early childhood. Psychological Science, 27(1), 34-42.
doi: 10.1177/0956797615607205URLpmid: 26573906
[68] Zeng,R., & Greenfield,P. M. (2015). Cultural evolution over the last 40 years in China: Using the Google Ngram Viewer to study implications of social and political change for cultural values. International Journal of Psychology, 50(1), 47-55.
doi: 10.1002/ijop.12125URLpmid: 25611928
[69] Zhang,R., & Weng,L. P. (2019). Not all cultural values are created equal: Cultural change in China reexamined through Google books. International Journal of Psychology, 54(1), 144-154.
doi: 10.1002/ijop.12436URLpmid: 28635144




[1]徐胜, 田蜜, 罗天瑞, 赛李阳. 基于选择反应的学习迁移效应:从个人情境到社会情境[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(7): 1215-1223.
[2]严瑜, 李彤. 工作场所不文明行为受害者向实施者反转的机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(7): 1307-1318.
[3]文鹏;史硕. 团队内非伦理行为的社会互动机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2012, 20(6): 805-814.
[4]庄锦英;徐璟;张森;余菲. 择偶复制:一种适应性的性选择策略[J]. 心理科学进展, 2012, 20(10): 1672-1678.
[5]阎书昌. 俄勒冈社会学习中心反社会行为预防与干预研究述评[J]. 心理科学进展, 2007, 15(04): 642-647.
[6]乐国安,张彦彦,陈浩. 西方有关择偶启事的研究和论争[J]. 心理科学进展, 2005, 13(02): 248-256.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=5262
相关话题/文化 心理 科学 社会 创新