删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

新生儿语音感知的神经基础:元分析

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

陈钰, 莫李澄, 毕蓉, 张丹丹()
深圳大学心理学院, 深圳 518060
收稿日期:2020-04-22出版日期:2020-08-15发布日期:2020-06-28
通讯作者:张丹丹E-mail:zhangdd05@gmail.com

基金资助:* 国家自然科学基金项目(31970980);深圳市基础研究自由探索项目(JCYJ20180305124305294);深港脑科学创新研究院(2019SHIBS0003)

The brain mechanism of speech perception in newborns: A meta-analysis

CHEN Yu, MO Licheng, BI Rong, ZHANG Dandan()
College of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China
Received:2020-04-22Online:2020-08-15Published:2020-06-28
Contact:ZHANG Dandan E-mail:zhangdd05@gmail.com






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 语言习得能力是人类在进化中获得的重要能力之一。语言认知的核心功能是语音加工, 因此, 语音加工的脑机制是认知心理学研究的重要课题。我们采用元分析方法, 对使用近红外技术考察新生儿语音感知的结构检测、偏差检测和母语感知的研究进行系统的定量分析, 探究新生儿的典型语音感知脑机制以及这三类语音加工相关脑区的异同。结果显示, 左侧额下回是新生儿检测语音结构的关键脑区; 双侧颞叶在新生儿语音偏差刺激检测中发挥关键作用; 新生儿的母语语音加工存在左侧化优势。



图1文献筛选流程
图1文献筛选流程


表1语音结构检测文献列表
文献 N 对比条件(contrast) 主要激活位置 效应量
Ferry et al., 2016 32 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额 0.66
Gervain et al., 2008 22 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额(左额激活最大) 0.33
Gervain et al., 2012 22 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额 0.34
Gervain et al., 2012 20 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额(左额激活最大) 0.37
Gómez et al., 2014 24 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额 0.39
Gómez et al., 2014 24 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额 0.41
Kudo et al., 2011 18 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额(左额激活最大) 0.54

表1语音结构检测文献列表
文献 N 对比条件(contrast) 主要激活位置 效应量
Ferry et al., 2016 32 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额 0.66
Gervain et al., 2008 22 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额(左额激活最大) 0.33
Gervain et al., 2012 22 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额 0.34
Gervain et al., 2012 20 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额(左额激活最大) 0.37
Gómez et al., 2014 24 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额 0.39
Gómez et al., 2014 24 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额 0.41
Kudo et al., 2011 18 特殊结构 > 一般结构 双侧颞-额(左额激活最大) 0.54


表2语音偏差刺激检测文献列表
文献 N 对比条件(contrast) 主要激活位置 效应量
Arimitsu et al., 2011 14 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.47
Carlier-Torres et al., 2014 13 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.51
Carlier-Torres et al., 2014 13 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.62
Mahmoudzadeh et al., 2013 6 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 1.03
Mahmoudzadeh et al., 2017 19 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.44
Sambeth, 2009 10 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.87

表2语音偏差刺激检测文献列表
文献 N 对比条件(contrast) 主要激活位置 效应量
Arimitsu et al., 2011 14 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.47
Carlier-Torres et al., 2014 13 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.51
Carlier-Torres et al., 2014 13 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.62
Mahmoudzadeh et al., 2013 6 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 1.03
Mahmoudzadeh et al., 2017 19 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.44
Sambeth, 2009 10 偏差刺激 > 标准刺激 双颞 0.87


表3母语感知文献列表
文献 N 对比条件(contrast) 主要激活位置 效应量
Pe?a et al., 2003 12 左颞 > 右颞 左颞 0.65
Sato et al., 2012 17 左颞 > 右颞 左颞 0.28
Vannasing et al., 2016 27 左颞 > 右颞 左颞 2.73

表3母语感知文献列表
文献 N 对比条件(contrast) 主要激活位置 效应量
Pe?a et al., 2003 12 左颞 > 右颞 左颞 0.65
Sato et al., 2012 17 左颞 > 右颞 左颞 0.28
Vannasing et al., 2016 27 左颞 > 右颞 左颞 2.73



图2使用fNIRS比较新生儿在检测语音序列结构时HbO2浓度的平均变化(contrast:特殊结构减一般结构)。Fisher’s Z, 效应值; CI, 置信区间。
图2使用fNIRS比较新生儿在检测语音序列结构时HbO2浓度的平均变化(contrast:特殊结构减一般结构)。Fisher’s Z, 效应值; CI, 置信区间。



图3使用fNIRS比较新生儿在检测语音偏差刺激时HbO2浓度的平均变化(contrast:偏差刺激减标准刺激)。Fisher’s Z, 效应值; CI, 置信区间。
图3使用fNIRS比较新生儿在检测语音偏差刺激时HbO2浓度的平均变化(contrast:偏差刺激减标准刺激)。Fisher’s Z, 效应值; CI, 置信区间。



图4使用fNIRS比较新生儿在感知母语语音刺激时HbO2浓度的平均变化(contrast:左半球减右半球)。Fisher’s Z, 效应值; CI, 置信区间。
图4使用fNIRS比较新生儿在感知母语语音刺激时HbO2浓度的平均变化(contrast:左半球减右半球)。Fisher’s Z, 效应值; CI, 置信区间。







*表示元分析用到的的文献
[1] 高晋健, 唐文渊. (1996). 近红外光谱仪在新生儿脑监测的临床应用. 国外医学(儿科学分册), 23(1), 7-10.
[2] 祁志强, 彭聃龄. (2010). 语音加工的脑机制研究: 现状、困惑及展望. 北京师范大学学报(社会科学版), (4), 40-47.
[3] 郑凤英, 彭少麟. (2001). Meta分析中几种常用效应值的介绍. 生态科学, 20(1, 2), 81-84.
[4] Abboub, N., Nazzi, T., & Gervain, J. (2016). Prosodic grouping at birth. Brain and Language, 162, 46-59.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.002URLpmid: 27567401
[5] Alamia, A., Solopchuk, O., D'Ausilio, A., van Bever, V., Fadiga, L., Olivier, E., & Zénon, A. (2016). Disruption of Broca's area alters higher-order chunking processing during perceptual sequence learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(3), 402-417.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00911URLpmid: 26765778
[6] *Arimitsu, T., Uchida-Ota, M., Yagihashi, T., Kojima, S., Watanabe, S., Hokuto, I., … Minagawa-Kawai, Y. (2011). Functional hemispheric specialization in processing phonemic and prosodic auditory changes in neonates. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 202.
URLpmid: 21954386
[7] Benavides-Varela, S., & Mehler, J. (2015). Verbal positional memory in 7-month-olds. Child Development, 86(1), 209-223.
URLpmid: 25176617
[8] Bhat, M., Palaniswamy, H. P., Pichaimuthu, A. N., & Thomas, N. (2018). Cortical auditory evoked potentials and hemispheric specialization of speech in individuals with learning disability and healthy controls: A preliminary study. F1000Research, 7, 1939.
URLpmid: 31001413
[9] Binder, J. R. (2017). Current controversies on Wernicke's area and its role in language. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 17(8), 58.
URLpmid: 28656532
[10] *Carlier-Torres, M. E. M., Harmony, T., Ricardo-garcell, J., Marroquin, J. L., & Colmenero, M. (2014). The hemodynamic response to acoustically modified syllables in premature and full term newborn infants acquired by near infrared spectroscopy. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 17(2), 13-21.
[11] Chen, S., Sun, Y., Ma, G., Yin, X., & Liang, L. (2019). The wedge insole for the treatment of knee osteoarthritis: A systematic review protocol. Medicine, 98(37), e17168.
URLpmid: 31517869
[12] Cheour, M., Martynova, O., N??t?nen, R., Erkkola, R., Sillanp??, M., Kero, P., … H?m?l?inen, H. (2002). Speech sounds learned by sleeping newborns. Nature, 415(6872), 599-600.
doi: 10.1038/415599aURLpmid: 11832929
[13] de Roever, I., Bale, G., Mitra, S., Meek, J., Robertson, N. J., & Tachtsidis, I. (2018). Investigation of the pattern of the hemodynamic response as measured by functional near-Infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) studies in newborns, less than a month old: A systematic review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 371.
URLpmid: 30333736
[14] Ferrari, M., & Quaresima, V. (2012). A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. NeuroImage, 63(2), 921-935.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.049URLpmid: 22510258
[15] *Ferry, A. L., Fló, A., Brusini, P., Cattarossi, L., Macagno, F., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J. . (2016). On the edge of language acquisition: Inherent constraints on encoding multisyllabic sequences in the neonate brain. Developmental Science, 19(3), 488-503.
[16] *Gervain, J., Berent, I., & Werker, J. F. (2012). Binding at birth: The newborn brain detects identity relations and sequential position in speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(3), 564-574.
URLpmid: 22066581
[17] *Gervain, J., Macagno, F., Cogoi, S., Pe?a, M., & Mehler, J. (2008). The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(37), 14222-14227.
[18] *Gómez, D. M., Berent, I., Benavides-Varela, S., Bion, R. A. H., Cattarossi, L., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (2014). Language universals at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(16), 5837-5841.
[19] *Kotilahti, K., Nissila, I., Nasi, T., Lipiainen, L., Noponen, T., Merilainen, P., … Fellman, V. (2010). Hemodynamic responses to speech and music in newborn infants. Human Brain Mapping, 31(4), 595-603.
URLpmid: 19790172
[20] Kudo, N., Nonaka, Y., Mizuno, N., Mizuno, K., & Okanoya, K. (2011). On-line statistical segmentation of a non-speech auditory stream in neonates as demonstrated by event-related brain potentials. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1100-1106.
URLpmid: 21884325
[21] Kujala, A., Huotilainen, M., Hotakainen, M., Lennes, M., Parkkonen, L., Fellman, V., & N??t?nen, R. (2004). Speech-sound discrimination in neonates as measured with MEG. Neuroreport, 15(13), 2089-2092.
URLpmid: 15486487
[22] *Mahmoudzadeh, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Fournier, M., Kongolo, G., Goudjil, S., Dubois, J., … Wallois, F. (2013). Syllabic discrimination in premature human infants prior to complete formation of cortical layers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(12), 4846-4851.
[23] *Mahmoudzadeh, M., Wallois, F., Kongolo, G., Goudjil, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2017). Functional maps at the onset of auditory inputs in very early preterm human neonates. Cerebral Cortex, 27(4), 2500-2512.
URLpmid: 27102655
[24] Marklund, E., Lacerda, F., & Schwarz, I.-C. (2018). Using rotated speech to approximate the acoustic mismatch negativity response to speech. Brain and Language, 176, 26-35.
URLpmid: 29172074
[25] May, L., Byers-Heinlein, K., Gervain, J., & Werker, J. F. (2011). Language and the newborn brain: does prenatal language experience shape the neonate neural response to speech? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 222.
URLpmid: 21960980
[26] Minagawa-Kawai, Y., van der Lely, H., Ramus, F., Sato, Y., Mazuka, R., & Dupoux, E. (2011). Optical brain imaging reveals general auditory and language-specific processing in early infant development. Cerebral Cortex, 21(2), 254-261.
URLpmid: 20497946
[27] Moon, C., Cooper, R. P., & Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 495-500.
[28] Paquette, N., Lassonde, M., Vannasing, P., Tremblay, J., González-Frankenberger, B., Florea, O., … Gallagher, A. (2015). Developmental patterns of expressive language hemispheric lateralization in children, adolescents and adults using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuropsychologia, 68, 117-125.
URLpmid: 25576910
[29] Partanen, E., Pakarinen, S., Kujala, T., & Huotilainen, M. (2013). Infants' brain responses for speech sound changes in fast multifeature MMN paradigm. Clinical Neurophysiology, 124(8), 1578-1585.
[30] *Pe?a, M., Maki, A., Kovacic, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F., & Mehler, J. (2003). Sounds and silence: an optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(20), 11702-11705.
[31] Pe?a, M., Pittaluga, E., & Mehler, J. (2010). Language acquisition in premature and full-term infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(8), 3823-3828.
[32] Perani, D., Saccuman, M. C., Scifo, P., Anwander, A., Spada, D., Baldoli, C., … Friederici, A. D. (2011). Neural language networks at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(38), 16056-16061.
[33] Planton, S., Chanoine, V., Sein, J., Anton, J.-L., Nazarian, B., Pallier, C., & Pattamadilok, C. (2019). Top-down activation of the visuo-orthographic system during spoken sentence processing. Neuroimage, 202, 116-135.
[34] Saarikivi, K., Putkinen, V., Tervaniemi, M., & Huotilainen, M. (2016). Cognitive flexibility modulates maturation and music- training-related changes in neural sound discrimination. European Journal of Neuroscience, 44(2), 1815-1825.
[35] Saito, Y., Kondo, T., Aoyama, S., Fukumoto, R., Konishi., N., Nakamura, K., … Toshima, T. (2007). The function of the frontal lobe in neonates for response to a prosodic voice. Early Human Development, 83(4), 225-230.
[36] *Sambeth, A., Pakarinen, S., Ruohio, K., Fellman, V., van Zuijen, T. L., & Huotilainen, M. (2009). Change detection in newborns using a multiple deviant paradigm: A study using magnetoencephalography. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(3), 530-538.
[37] *Sato, H., Hirabayashi, Y., Tsubokura, H., Kanai, M., Ashida, T., Konishi, I., … Maki, A. (2012). Cerebral hemodynamics in newborn infants exposed to speech sounds: A whole-head optical topography study. Human Brain Mapping, 33(9), 2092-2103.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.21350URLpmid: 21714036
[38] Schroeder, S. R., & Rembrandt, H. N. (2018). How effectively do people remember voice disordered speech? An investigation of the serial-position curve. Brain Sciences, 8( 2), 25.
doi: 10.3390/brainsci8120211URLpmid: 30513678
[39] Telkemeyer, S., Rossi, S., Koch, S. P., Nierhaus, T., Steinbrink, J., Poeppel, D., … Wartenburger, I. (2009). Sensitivity of newborn auditory cortex to the temporal structure of sounds. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(47), 14726-14733.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1246-09.2009URLpmid: 19940167
[40] Uddén, J., Ingvar, M., Hagoort, P., & Petersson, K. M. (2017). Broca's region: A causal role in implicit processing of grammars with crossed non-adjacent dependencies. Cognition, 164, 188-198.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.03.010URLpmid: 28453996
[41] *Vannasing, P., Florea, O., González-Frankenberger, B., Tremblay, J., Paquette, N., Safi, D., … Gallagher, A. (2016). Distinct hemispheric specializations for native and non-native languages in one-day-old newborns identified by fNIRS. Neuropsychologia, 84, 63-69.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.038URLpmid: 26851309
[42] von Holzen, K., Nishibayashi, L. L., Nazzi, T. (2018). Consonant and vowel processing in word form segmentation: An infant ERP study. Brain Sciences, 8(2), 24.
[43] Wenrich, K. A., Davidson, L. S., & Uchanski, R. M. (2017). Segmental and suprasegmental perception in children using hearing aids. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 28(10), 901-912.
doi: 10.3766/jaaa.16105URLpmid: 29130438
[44] Zhang, D. D., Chen, Y., Hou, X. L., & Wu, Y. J. (2019). Near-infrared spectroscopy reveals neural perception of vocal emotions in human neonates. Human Brain Mapping, 40(8), 2434-2448.
URLpmid: 30697881
[45] Zhang, H. J., Dong, W. T., Dang, W. M., Quan, W. X., Tian, J., Chen, R., … Yu, X. (2015). Near-infrared spectroscopy for examination of prefrontal activation during cognitive tasks in patients with major depressive disorder: A meta- analysis of observational studies. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 69(1), 22-33.
doi: 10.1111/pcn.12209URLpmid: 24897940




[1]曾宪卿, 许冰, 孙博, 叶健彤, 傅世敏. EMMN受偏差-标准刺激对类型和情绪类型影响: 来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1163-1178.
[2]张雯, 胡娜, 丁雪辰, 李俊一. 拒绝敏感性与边缘型人格特征的关联:一项元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1179-1194.
[3]叶静, 张戌凡. 老年人心理韧性与幸福感的关系:一项元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(2): 202-217.
[4]黄崇蓉, 胡瑜. 组织内信任与创造力的关系:元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(7): 1118-1132.
[5]张建平, 秦传燕, 刘善仕. 寻求反馈能改善绩效吗?——反馈寻求行为与个体绩效关系的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(4): 549-565.
[6]方俊燕, 张敏强. 元回归中效应量的最小个数需求:基于统计功效和估计精度[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(4): 673-680.
[7]张亚利, 李森, 俞国良. 孤独感和手机成瘾的关系:一项元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(11): 1836-1852.
[8]胥彦, 李超平. 人口统计学特征对公共服务动机有什么影响?来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(10): 1631-1649.
[9]丁凤琴, 王冬霞. 道德概念具身隐喻及其影响因素:来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(9): 1540-1555.
[10]胥彦, 李超平. 领导风格与敬业度关系的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(8): 1363-1383.
[11]辛素飞, 姜文源, 辛自强. 1993至2016年医学生心理健康变迁的横断历史研究[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(7): 1183-1193.
[12]张亚利, 李森, 俞国良. 自尊与社交焦虑的关系:基于中国学生群体的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(6): 1005-1018.
[13]曹艺, 杨小虎. 精神分裂症患者的语音感知[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(6): 1025-1035.
[14]汤明, 李伟强, 刘福会, 袁博. 内疚与亲社会行为的关系:来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(5): 773-788.
[15]邵嵘, 滕召军, 刘衍玲. 暴力视频游戏对个体亲社会性的影响:一项元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(3): 453-464.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=5129
相关话题/结构 文献 心理 科学 流程