删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

驾驶员为何对道路关键事件“视而不见”?——“自下而上”和“自上而下”加工机制的交互作用

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

袁璐一, 常若松, 马锦飞()
辽宁师范大学心理学院, 大连 116029
收稿日期:2018-06-24出版日期:2019-03-15发布日期:2019-01-22
通讯作者:马锦飞E-mail:majinfei666@126.com

基金资助:辽宁省博士科研启动基金项目“车载广播对汽车驾驶员警觉的影响机制研究”(201601242);辽宁省教育厅人文社科项目“驾驶员归因倾向对攻击性驾驶行为的影响”(W201683617)

Why does a driver can not see a critical event on the road?Interaction between “bottom-up” and “top-down” processing mechanisms

YUAN Luyi, CHANG Ruosong, MA Jinfei()
School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
Received:2018-06-24Online:2019-03-15Published:2019-01-22
Contact:MA Jinfei E-mail:majinfei666@126.com






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 驾驶员“视而不见”错误是指驾驶员的视线虽然指向道路上的危险目标, 却没有知觉到其存在, 对交通安全构成威胁。本文首先在注意计算框架内, 分别从“自下而上”和“自上而下”两种加工机制探讨诱发驾驶员“视而不见”错误的主要因素, 该框架解释力不足的原因在于没有充分考虑实践经验和动机对驾驶员预期和注意定势的影响。由此提出注意计算框架扩展模型, 通过“自下而上”和“自上而下”的交互作用机制对驾驶员“视而不见”错误提出综合解释。



图1驾驶员注意计算框架扩展模型(改编自Gu等, 2005)
图1驾驶员注意计算框架扩展模型(改编自Gu等, 2005)







[1] 张慧, 施建农 . ( 2014). 无意视盲的认知神经机制. 心理科学进展, 22(12), 1867-1874.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01867
[2] Agrawal R., Knodler M., Fisher D. L., & Samuel S . ( 2017, October). Advanced virtual reality based training to improve young drivers' latent hazard anticipation ability. Paper presented at the meeting of Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. Massachusetts.
[3] Atchley P., Tran A. V., & Salehinejad M. A . ( 2017). Constructing a publically available distracted driving database and research tool. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 99, 306-311.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2016.12.005URLpmid: 28024203
[4] Beanland V., Filtness A. J., & Jeans R . ( 2017). Change detection in urban and rural driving scenes: Effects of target type and safety relevance on change blindness. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 100, 111-122.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2017.01.011URLpmid: 28130981
[5] Beanland V., Lenné M. G., & R??ger L . ( 2015). Psychological factors in seeing motorcycles. In L. R??ger, M. G. Lenné, & G. Underwood (Eds.), Increasing motorcycle conspicuity: Design and assessment of interventions to enhance rider safety( pp. 21-50). Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.
[6] Beanland V., Lenné M. G., & Underwood G . ( 2014). Safety in numbers: Target prevalence affects the detection of vehicles during simulated driving. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 76( 3), 805-813.
[7] Bergen B., Medeiros-ward N., Wheeler K., Drews F., & Strayer D . ( 2013). The crosstalk hypothesis: Why language interferes with driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142( 1), 119-130.
doi: 10.1037/a0028428URLpmid: 22612769
[8] Borowsky A., Shinar D., & Parmet Y . ( 2008). The relation between driving experience and recognition of road signs relative to their locations. Human Factors, 50( 2), 173-182.
doi: 10.1518/001872008X288330URLpmid: 18516830
[9] Briggs G. F., Hole G. J & Turner J. A. J. .,( 2018). The impact of attentional set and situation awareness on dual tasking driving performance. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology & Behaviour, 57, 36-47.
[10] Briggs G. F., Hole G. J., & Land M. F . ( 2016). Imagery-inducing distraction leads to cognitive tunnelling and deteriorated driving performance. Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour, 38, 106-117.
doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2016.01.007URL
[11] Cartwright-finch U. & Lavie N., ( 2007). The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition, 102( 3), 321-340.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.002URLpmid: 16480973
[12] Cassarino M. & Setti A., ( 2016). Complexity as key to designing cognitive-friendly environments for older people. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1329.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01329URL
[13] Cavallo V., Ranchet M., Pinto M., Espié S., Vienne F., & Dang N-T . ( 2015). Improving car drivers' perception of motorcycle motion through innovative headlight configurations. Accident Analysis & Prevention 81, 187-193.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2015.04.034URLpmid: 25988810
[14] Chan E., Pradhan A. K., Pollatsek A., Knodler M. A., & Fisher D. L . ( 2010). Are driving simulators effective tools for evaluating novice drivers’ hazard anticipation, speed management, and attention maintenance skills. Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour, 13( 5), 343-353.
doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2010.04.001URLpmid: 20729986
[15] Charlton S.G., & Starkey N.J . ( 2013). Driving on familiar roads: Automaticity and inattention blindness. Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour, 19, 121-133.
doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2013.03.008URL
[16] Ciceri M.R., & Ruscio D. , ( 2014). Does driving experience in video games count? Hazard anticipation and visual exploration of male gamers as function of driving experience. Transportation Research Part F: Psychology & Behaviou, 22, 76-85.
doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2013.11.001URL
[17] Clabaux N., Brenac T., Perrin C., Magnin J., Canu B., &Van Elslande P . ( 2012). Motorcyclists' speed and "looked-but-failed-to-see" accidents. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 49, 73-77.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.07.013URLpmid: 23036384
[18] Clarke D. D., Ward P., Bartle C., & Truman W . ( 2007). The role of motorcyclist and other driver behaviour in two types of serious accident in the UK. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 39( 5), 974-981.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2007.01.002pmid: 17854573
[19] Costa M., Simone A., Vignali V., Lantieri C., Bucchi A., .. Dondi G . ( 2014). Looking behavior for vertical road signs. Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour, 23, 147-155.
doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2014.01.003URL
[20] Crundall D., Clarke D., Ward P., & Bartle C . ( 2008). Car drivers' skills and attitudes to motorcycle safety: A review. Road Safety Research Report NO.85. Department for Transport, London.
[21] Crundall D., Clarke D., & Shahar A . ( 2011). Car drivers' attitudes and visual skills in relation to motorcyclists. Road Safety Research Report No. 121. Department for Transport, London.
[22] Crundall D., Crundall E., Clarke D., & Shahar A . ( 2012). Why do car drivers fail to give way to motorcycles at t-junctions? Accident Analysis & Prevention, 44( 1), 88-96.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2010.10.017URLpmid: 22062341
[23] Crundall D., Howard A., & Young A . ( 2017). Perceptual training to increase drivers’ ability to spot motorcycles at T-junctions. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology & Behaviour, 48, 1-12.
[24] de Craen S., Doumen M. J. A., & van Norden Y . ( 2014). A different perspective on conspicuity related motorcycle crashes. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 63, 133-137.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2013.10.027URLpmid: 24291070
[25] Department for Transport . ( 2016). Reported road casualties Great Britain: main results: 2015 Retrieved from. on14/10/16.
[26] Dingus T. A., Guo F., Lee S., Antin J. F., Perez M., Buchanan-King M., .. Hankey J . ( 2016). Driver crash risk factors and prevalence evaluation using naturalistic driving data. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113( 10), 2636-2641.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1513271113URLpmid: 26903657
[27] Engstr?m J., Markkula G., Victor T., & Merat N . ( 2017). Effects of cognitive load on driving performance: The cognitive control hypothesis. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 59( 5), 734-764.
doi: 10.1177/0018720817690639URLpmid: 28186421
[28] Fisher D. L., Pradhan A., Pollatsek A., & Jr Knodler M . ( 2007, December). Empirical evaluation of hazard anticipation behaviors in the field and on driving simulator using eye tracker. Paper presented at The 86th Transportation Research Board annual meetings. Washington, DC.
[29] Fisher D.L., & Strayer D.L . ( 2014). Modeling situation awareness and crash risk. Annals of Advances in Automotive Medicine, 58, 33-39.
URLpmid: 24776225
[30] Gershon P., Ben-asher N., & Shinar D . ( 2012). Attention and search conspicuity of motorcycles as a function of their visual context. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 44( 1), 97-103.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2010.12.015URLpmid: 22062342
[31] Gibbs R., Davies G., & Chou S . ( 2016). A systematic review on factors affecting the likelihood of change blindness. Crime Psychology Review, 2( 1), 1-21, DOI: 10.1080/23744006.2016.1228799.
doi: 10.1080/23744006.2016.1228799URL
[32] Greene C. M., Murphy G., & Januszewski J . ( 2017). Under high perceptual load, observers look but do not see. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 31( 4). DOI: 10.1002/acp.3335.
doi: 10.1002/acp.3335URL
[33] Green M. ( 2004). Inattentional blindness and conspicuity. Retrieved November 10.
[34] Gu E., Stocker C., & Badler N. I . ( 2005). Do you see what eyes see? Implementing inattentional blindness. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 3661, 178-190.
doi: 10.1007/11550617_16URL
[35] Hancock P. A., Wulf G., Thom D., & Fassnacht P . ( 1990). Driver workload during differing driving maneuvers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 22( 3), 281-290.
doi: 10.1016/0001-4575(90)90019-HURLpmid: 2393475
[36] Harvey A. J., Bayless S. J., & Hyams G . ( 2018). Alcohol increases inattentional blindness when cognitive resources are not consumed by ongoing task demands. Psychopharmacology, 235( 1), 309-315.
doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4772-9URL
[37] Heenan A., Herdman C. M., Brown M. S., & Robert N . ( 2014). Effects of conversation on situation awareness and working memory in simulated driving. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 56( 6), 1077-1092.
doi: 10.1177/0018720813519265URLpmid: 25277018
[38] Hole G. J., Tyrrell L., Langham M . ( 1996). Some factors affecting motorcyclists' conspicuity. Ergonomics, 39( 7), 946-965.
doi: 10.1080/00140139608964516URL
[39] Jacobsen P. L., Ragland D. R., & Komanoff C . ( 2015). Safety in numbers for walkers and bicyclists: Exploring the mechanisms. Injury Prevention Journal of the International Society for Child & Adolescent Injury Prevention, 21( 4), 217-220.
doi: 10.1136/injuryprev-2015-041635URLpmid: 26157109
[40] Jehring J. ( 1931). Industrial accident prevention: A scientific approach by H. W. Heinrich. Industrial & Labor Relations Review, 4( 4), 609.
[41] Koivisto M., Hy?n? J., & Revonsuo A . ( 2004). The effects of eye movements, spatial attention, and stimulus features on inattentional blindness. Vision Research, 44( 27), 3211-3221.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.07.026URLpmid: 15482807
[42] Koornstra M. Bijleveld F. & Hagenzicker M. ,( 1997). The safety effects of daytime running lights: A perspective on daytime running lights (DRL) in the EU. ( Tech Rep. R-97-36). The Netherlands: SWOV Institute for Road Safety Research.
[43] Kreitz C., Schnuerch R., Furley P. A., & Memmert D . ( 2018). What’s past is past: Neither perceptual preactivation nor prior motivational relevance decrease subsequent inattentional blindness. Consciousness and Cognition, 59, 1-9.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.01.003URL
[44] Land M.F., & Furneaux S. , ( 1997). The knowledge base of the oculomotor system. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 352( 1358), 1231-1239.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0105URLpmid: 1692006
[45] Langham M.P . ( 1999). An investigation of the role of vehicle conspicuity in the 'looked but failed to see' error in driving (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). University of Sussex, UK.
[46] Langham M., Hole G., Edwards J., & O'Neil C . ( 2002). An analysis of 'looked but failed to see' accidents involving parked police vehicles. Ergonomics, 45( 3), 167-185.
doi: 10.1080/00140130110115363URLpmid: 11964202
[47] Lavie N. ( 2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9( 2), 75-82.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004URLpmid: 15668100
[48] Law T. H., Ghanbari M., Hamid H., Abdul-Halin A., & Ng C. P . ( 2016). Role of sensory and cognitive conspicuity in the prevention of collisions between motorcycles and trucks at T-intersections. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 96, 64-70.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2016.04.013URLpmid: 27505097
[49] Lee Y. M., Sheppard E., & Crundall D . ( 2015). Cross- cultural effects on the perception and appraisal of approaching motorcycles at junctions. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 31, 77-86.
doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2015.03.013URL
[50] Mack A. & Rock I., ( 1998). Inattentional Blindness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2( 5), 3244.
[51] MartensM. M.H . ( 2011). Change detection in traffic: Where do we look and what do we perceive? Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 14( 3), 240-250.
doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2011.01.004URL
[52] Martens M. H .(2017). The failure to respond to changes in the road environment: Does road familiarity play a role? Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour.DOI: 10.1016/j.trf. 2017. 08. 003.
[53] Mccarley J. S., Steelman K. S., & Horrey W. J . ( 2014). The view from the driver's seat: What good is salience? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28( 1), 47-54.
doi: 10.1002/acp.2955URL
[54] Most S.B., & Astur R.S . ( 2007). Feature-based attentional set as a cause of traffic accidents. Visual Cognition, 15( 2), 125-132.
doi: 10.1080/13506280600959316URL
[55] Murphy G., & Greene C.M . ( 2015). High perceptual load causes inattentional blindness and deafness in drivers. Visual Cognition, 23( 7), 810-814.
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2015.1093245URL
[56] Musselwhite C. B. A., Avineri E., Susilo Y. O., & Bhattachary D . ( 2012). Public attitudes towards motorcyclists' safety: A qualitative study from the United Kingdom. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 49, 105-113.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.06.005URLpmid: 23036387
[57] Pammer K., Bairnsfather J., Burns J., & Hellsing A . ( 2015). Not all hazards are created equal: The significance of hazards in inattentional blindness for static driving scenes. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29( 5), 782-788.
doi: 10.1002/acp.3153URL
[58] Pammer K. & Blink C., ( 2013). Attentional differences in driving judgments for country and city scenes: Semantic congruency in inattentional blindness. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 50, 955-963.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.07.026URLpmid: 22975367
[59] Parthemore J., & Morse A.F . ( 2010). Representations reclaimed: Accounting for the co-emergence of concepts and experience. Pragmatics & Cognition, 18( 2), 273-312.
[60] Pringle H. L., Irwin D. E., Kramer A. F., & Atchley P . ( 2001). The role of attentional breadth in perceptual change detection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8( 1), 89-95.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196143URLpmid: 11340871
[61] S?nger J. & Wascher E., ( 2011). The influence of extrinsic motivation on competition-based selection. Behavioural Brain Research, 224( 1), 58-64.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.05.015URLpmid: 21645552
[62] Shahar A., van Loon E., Clarke D., & Crundall D . ( 2012). Attending overtaking cars and motorcycles through the mirrors before changing lanes. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 44( 1), 104-110.
doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.01.001URLpmid: 22062343
[63] Shiferaw B., Stough C., & Downey L . ( 2014). Drivers' visual scanning impairment under the influences of alcohol and distraction: A literature review. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 7( 3), 174-182.
[64] Shinoda H., Hayhoe M. M., & Shrivastava A . ( 2001). What controls attention in natural environments? Vision Research, 41( 25-26), 3535-3545.
doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00199-7URLpmid: 11718793
[65] Simons D. J, Chabris C. F . ( 1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28( 9), 1059-1074.
doi: 10.1068/p281059URLpmid: 10694957
[66] Son J. & Park M., ( 2017). Situation awareness and transitions in highly automated driving: A framework and mini review. Journal of Ergonomics, 7(5), 212. DOI: 10.4172/2165-7556.1000212.
[67] Strayer D.L., & Fisher D.L . ( 2015). SPIDER: A framework for understanding driver distraction. Human Factors, 58( 1), 5-12.
doi: 10.1177/0018720815619074URLpmid: 26715688
[68] SWOV. ( 2017). Motorcyclists: SWOV-Factsheet. SWOV Institute for Road Safety Research, Leidschendam, The Netherlands.
[69] Thomson G.A . ( 1980). The role frontal motorcycle conspicuity has in road accidents. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 12( 3), 165-178.
doi: 10.1016/0001-4575(80)90015-9URL
[70] Torrez L. ( 2008). Motorcycle conspicuity: Effects of age and daytime running lights, Human Factors, 52( 3), 355-369.
doi: 10.1177/0018720810374613URLpmid: 21077560
[71] Ward E.J., & Scholl B.J . ( 2015). Inattentional blindness reflects limitations on perception, not memory: Evidence from repeated failures of awareness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22( 3), 722-727.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0745-8URLpmid: 25515671
[72] Wells S., Mullin B., Norton R., Langley J., Connor J., Lay-yee R., & Jackson R . ( 2004). Motorcycle rider conspicuity and crash related injury: Case-control study. British Medical Journal, 328( 7444), 857.
doi: 10.1136/bmj.37984.574757.EEURLpmid: 14742349
[73] Wulf G., Hancock P. A., & Rahimi M . ( 1989). Motorcycle conspicuity: An evaluation and synthesis of influential factors. Journal of Safety Research, 20( 4), 153-176.
doi: 10.1016/0022-4375(89)90025-XURL




[1]戴梦诺, 李艳菊, 杜峰. 多重注意定势的证据:来自空间瞬脱的结果[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(suppl.): 9-9.
[2]张慧;施建农. 无意视盲的认知神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(12): 1867-1874.
[3]张明;王凌云. 注意瞬脱的瓶颈理论[J]. 心理科学进展, 2009, 17(1): 7-16.
[4]李会杰;陈楚侨. 注意捕获的另一扇窗户——无意视盲[J]. 心理科学进展, 2007, 15(4): 577-586.
[5]储衡清,周晓林. 注意捕获与自上而下的加工过程[J]. 心理科学进展, 2004, 12(5): 680-687.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4627
相关话题/科学 心理 计算 神经 科研