武汉大学哲学学院心理学系, 武汉 430072
收稿日期:
2017-07-29出版日期:
2018-07-15发布日期:
2018-05-29通讯作者:
谢天E-mail:thanksky520@126.com基金资助:
国家社会科学基金教育学青年课题的资助(课题名称:物质主义的成因分析及其教育对策)(CBA150154)The dynamic process of social norm development
CHEN Weiyang, XIE Tian()Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Wuhan University, Wuhan 430072, China
Received:
2017-07-29Online:
2018-07-15Published:
2018-05-29Contact:
XIE Tian E-mail:thanksky520@126.com摘要/Abstract
摘要: 社会心理学的经典研究将社会规范区分为描述性规范与指令性规范。近年来, 研究者从以往对社会规范的静态区分, 开始深入研究社会规范“如何发展变化”的动态过程, 即探究社会规范的习得、传播与转变过程, 以及社会规范发挥作用的影响因素。社会规范的习得可以追溯至童年早期的社会互动, 描述性规范的形成是个体在社会网络中学习社会范畴典型范例的过程, 对现状的合理化催生了描述性规范向指令性规范的转化。社会规范发挥作用也呈现动态性, 不仅与社会规范的心理功能相关, 即遵守或违反社会规范能满足个体的安全感、归属感、独特感需要等, 还与外部环境如权力位置、惩罚机制相关。未来研究可对社会规范形成过程、作用过程做进一步分析; 结合社会规范强度研究, 深入探讨规范性影响的作用过程; 开展文化情境下的社会规范研究等。
参考文献 47
1 | 陈思静, 何铨, 马剑虹 . ( 2015). 第三方惩罚对合作行为的影响: 基于社会规范激活的解释. 心理学报, 47( 3), 389-405. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00389URL |
2 | 卢俊, 陈浩, 乐国安 . ( 2017). 松-紧文化: 跨文化心理学研究的新维度. 心理科学进展, 25( 5), 887-902. |
3 | 汪新建, 吕小康 . ( 2009). 作为惯习的潜规则——潜规则盛行的文化心理学分析框架. 南开学报(哲学社会科学版), ( 4), 133-139. |
4 | 吴思 . ( 2001). 潜规则: 中国历史中的真实游戏. 昆明: 云南人民出版社. |
5 | 谢天, 石双 . ( 2016). 比较视角: 幸福感与物质主义、传统规范的交互影响. 黑龙江社会科学, ( 2), 96-102. |
6 | Anderson C., Hildreth J. A. D., & Howland L . ( 2015). Is the desire for status a fundamental human motive? A review of the empirical literature. Psychological Bulletin, 141( 3), 574-601. doi: 10.1037/a0038781URLpmid: 25774679 |
7 | Andrighetto G., Brandts J., Conte R., Sabater-Mir J., Solaz H., & Villatoro D . ( 2013). Punish and voice: Punishment enhances cooperation when combined with norm-signalling. PLoS One, 8( 6), e64941. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064941URLpmid: 3680389 |
8 | Blake P. R., McAuliffe K., Corbit J., Callaghan T. C., Barry O., Bowie A., .. Warneken F . ( 2015). The ontogeny of fairness in seven societies. Nature, 528( 7581), 258-261. doi: 10.1038/nature15703URLpmid: 26580018 |
9 | Bowles, H. R., & Gelfand, M . ( 2010). Status and the evaluation of workplace deviance. Psychological Science, 21( 1), 49-54. doi: 10.1177/0956797609356509URLpmid: 20424022 |
10 | Brewer, M. B . ( 1991). The social self: On being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17( 5), 475-482. doi: 10.1177/0146167291175001URL |
11 | Castano, E . ( 2004). In case of death, cling to the ingroup. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34( 4), 375-384. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.211URL |
12 | Chao M. M., Zhang Z. X., & Chiu C. Y . ( 2010). Adherence to perceived norms across cultural boundaries: The role of need for cognitive closure and ingroup identification. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13( 1), 69-89. doi: 10.1177/1368430209343115URL |
13 | Chen, Y. Y., & Hong, Y. Y . ( 2015). Different ways to resolve discrepancy between descriptive and injunctive norms across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46( 10), 1316-1319. doi: 10.1177/0022022115600265URL |
14 | Cialdini R. B., Kallgren C. A., & Reno R. R . ( 1991). A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 201-234. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60330-5URL |
15 | Cialdini R. B., Martin S. J., & Goldstein N. J . ( 2015). Small behavioral science-informed changes can produce large policy-relevant effects. Behavioral Science & Policy, 1( 1), 21-27. doi: 10.1353/bsp.2015.0008URL |
16 | Eom K., Kim H. S., Sherman D. K., & Ishii K . ( 2016). Cultural variability in the link between environmental concern and support for environmental action. Psychological Science, 27( 10), 1331-1339. doi: 10.1177/0956797616660078URLpmid: 27565535 |
17 | Eriksson K., Strimling P., & Coultas J. C . ( 2015). Bidirectional associations between descriptive and injunctive norms. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 59-69. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.09.011URL |
18 | Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U . ( 2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25( 2), 63-87. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4URL |
19 | Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. ( 2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415( 6868), 137-140. doi: 10.1038/415137aURL |
20 | Galinsky A. D., Magee J. C., Gruenfeld D. H., Whitson J. A., & Liljenquist K. A . ( 2008). Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95( 6), 1450-1466. doi: 10.1037/a0012633URL |
21 | Gao W. J., Qiu L., Chiu C. Y., & Yang Y. Y . ( 2015). Diffusion of opinions in a complex culture system: Implications for emergence of descriptive norms. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46( 10), 1252-1259. doi: 10.1177/0022022115610212URL |
22 | Gelfand, M. J., & Jackson, J. C . ( 2016). From one mind to many: The emerging science of cultural norms. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 175-181. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.11.002URL |
23 | Gelfand M. J., Raver J. L., Nishii L., Leslie L. M., Lun J., Lim B. C., .. Yamaguchi S . ( 2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332( 6033), 1100-1104. doi: 10.1126/science.1197754URLpmid: 21617077 |
24 | GÖckeritz S., Schmidt M. F. H., & Tomasello M . ( 2014). Young children's creation and transmission of social norms. Cognitive Development, 30, 81-95. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.01.003URL |
25 | Harrington, J. R., & Gelfand, M. J . ( 2014). Tightness- looseness across the 50 united states. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111( 22), 7990-7995. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1317937111URLpmid: 24843116 |
26 | Jost J. T., Sterling J. L., & Langer M . ( 2015). From “Is” to “Ought” and sometimes “Not”: Compliance with and resistance to social norms from a system justification perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46( 10), 1287-1291. doi: 10.1177/0022022115600268URL |
27 | Kashima E. S., Halloran M., Yuki M., & Kashima Y . ( 2004). The effects of personal and collective mortality salience on individualism: Comparing Australians and Japanese with higher and lower self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40( 3), 384-392. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.07.007URL |
28 | Kashima Y., Wilson S., Lusher D., Pearson L. J., & Pearson C . ( 2013). The acquisition of perceived descriptive norms as social category learning in social networks. Social Networks, 35( 4), 711-719. doi: 10.1016/j.socnet.2013.06.002URL |
29 | Kosic A., Kruglanski A. W., Pierro A., & Mannetti L . ( 2004). The social cognition of immigrants’ acculturation: Effects of the need for closure and the reference group at entry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86( 6), 796-813. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.6.796URLpmid: 15149256 |
30 | Kreuzbauer R., Chiu C. Y., Lin S. D., & Bae S. H . ( 2014). When does life satisfaction accompany relational identity signaling: A cross-cultural analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45( 4), 646-659. doi: 10.1177/0022022113518369URL |
31 | Kruglanski A. W. ( 2004). The psychology of closed mindedness. New York, NY: Psychology Press. |
32 | Kwan L. Y. Y., Yap S., & Chiu C. Y . ( 2015). Mere exposure affects perceived descriptive norms: Implications for personal preferences and trust. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 48-58. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.12.002URL |
33 | Livi S., Kruglanski A. W., Pierro A., Mannetti L., & Kenny D. A . ( 2015). Epistemic motivation and perpetuation of group culture: Effects of need for cognitive closure on trans- generational norm transmission. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 105-112. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.09.010URL |
34 | McAuliffe K., Jordan J. J., & Warneken F . ( 2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 1-10. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.013URLpmid: 25460374 |
35 | Morris M. W., Chiu C. Y., & Liu Z . ( 2015). Polycultural psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 631-659. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015001URL |
36 | Morris, M. W., & Liu, Z . ( 2015). Psychological functions of subjective norms: Reference groups, moralization, adherence, and defiance. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46( 10), 1279-1287. doi: 10.1177/0022022115612161URL |
37 | Mulder, L. B . ( 2008). The difference between punishments and rewards in fostering moral concerns in social decision making. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44( 6), 1436-1443. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.06.004URL |
38 | Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E . ( 2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26( 4), 269-281. doi: 10.1111/1471-6402.t01-1-00066URL |
39 | Roos P., Gelfand M., Nau D., & Lun J . ( 2015). Societal threat and cultural variation in the strength of social norms: An evolutionary basis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 14-23. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2015.01.003URL |
40 | Savani K., Wadhwa M., Uchida Y., Ding Y., & Naidu, N. V. R . ( 2015). When norms loom larger than the self: Susceptibility of preference-choice consistency to normative influence across cultures. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 70-79. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.09.001URL |
41 | Stamkou E., van Kleef G. A., Homan A. C., & Galinsky A. D . ( 2016). How norm violations shape social hierarchies: Those who stand on top block norm violators from rising up. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 19( 5), 608-629. doi: 10.1177/1368430216641305URL |
42 | van Kleef G. A., Homan A. C., Finkenauer C., Blaker N. M., & Heerdink M. W . ( 2012). Prosocial norm violations fuel power affordance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48( 4), 937-942. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.022URL |
43 | van Kleef G. A., Homan A. C., Finkenauer C., Gündemir S., & Stamkou E . ( 2011). Breaking the rules to rise to power: How norm violators gain power in the eyes of others. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2( 5), 500-507. doi: 10.1177/1948550611398416URL |
44 | Wan, C . ( 2015). Understanding cultural identification through intersubjective cultural representation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46( 10), 1267-1272. doi: 10.1177/0022022115610213URL |
45 | Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M . ( 2013). The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116( 2), 338-350. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.002URLpmid: 23917162 |
46 | Williams, A., & Moore, C . ( 2016). A longitudinal exploration of advantageous and disadvantageous inequality aversion in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 294-306. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.006URLpmid: 27632711 |
47 | Xie W. W., Ho B., Meier S., & Zhou X. Y . ( 2017). Rank reversal aversion inhibits redistribution across societies. Nature Human Behavior, 1( 8), 0142. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0142URL |
相关文章 15
[1] | 张辉华. 社会网络视角的团队情绪智力[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1381-1395. |
[2] | 崔馨月, 李斌, 贺汝婉, 张淑颖, 雷励. 亲社会支出对主观幸福感的影响及其作用机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1279-1290. |
[3] | 王晓田, 王娜, 何金波. 前瞻性情绪作为社会风险的信息源假说:公共场景下风险决策的情绪及文化机制探讨[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 959-966. |
[4] | 李铭泽, 叶慧莉, 张光磊. 自恋型领导对团队创造力形成过程的多视角研究[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(9): 1437-1453. |
[5] | 欧红蕾, 孙炳海, 张文海, 李伟健. 勿以恶小而为之:滑坡效应的心理机制及预防策略[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(4): 650-660. |
[6] | 陈晨, 刘玉新, 赵晨. 独立、协同和平衡视角下的单维式基本心理需要满足[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(12): 2076-2090. |
[7] | 张春虎. 基于自我决定理论的工作动机研究脉络及未来走向[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(8): 1489-1506. |
[8] | 吴才智, 荣硕, 朱芳婷, 谌燕, 郭永玉. 基本心理需要及其满足 *[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(6): 1063-1073. |
[9] | 卢俊;陈浩;乐国安. 松−紧文化: 跨文化心理学研究的新维度[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(5): 887-902. |
[10] | 邓小平, 徐晨, 程懋伟, 张向葵. 青少年偏差行为的同伴选择和影响效应:基于纵向社会网络的元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(11): 1898-1909. |
[11] | 尹奎;李秀凤;孙健敏;于浩瀛. 大学生的社会网络[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(8): 1279-1289. |
[12] | 张镇;郭博达. 社会网络视角下的同伴关系与心理健康[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(4): 591-602. |
[13] | 葛红宁;周宗奎;牛更枫;陈武. 社交网站使用能带来社会资本吗?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(3): 454-463. |
[14] | 赵丹;余林. 社会交往对老年人认知功能的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(1): 46-54. |
[15] | 李惠璠;范秀成;曹花蕊;杜建刚. 市场规范与社会规范冲突视角下的奖励推荐计划作用机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(1): 11-21. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4381