深圳大学心理与社会学院, 深圳 518060
收稿日期:
2017-05-23出版日期:
2018-06-10发布日期:
2018-04-28通讯作者:
李红E-mail:lihongszu@szu.edu.cn基金资助:
* 国家自然科学基金项目(31671150);广东省普通高校创新团队建设项目(2015KCXTD009);广东省(基础研究及应用研究)重大项目(2016KZDXM009);深圳市2015年基础研究布局项目(34255/00001)The psychological mechanism of enjoying sad music
WANG Ding, WANG Chao, LI Hong()College of Psychology and Sociology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China
Received:
2017-05-23Online:
2018-06-10Published:
2018-04-28Contact:
LI Hong E-mail:lihongszu@szu.edu.cn摘要/Abstract
摘要: 悲伤音乐在唤起悲伤情绪的同时也可以唤起愉悦感或带来继发的心理获益。基于此, 研究者们提出了悲伤音乐唤起愉悦感的“解离理论”和“中和理论”, 以阐述人们喜欢悲伤音乐的心理机制。然而现有理论仍不足以提供充分合理的解释, 因此从神经生物学角度探索悲伤音乐唤起愉悦感的神经机制从而揭示现有心理学理论的神经基础是未来研究的方向。
参考文献 72
[1] | 白学军, 马谐, 陶云 . ( 2016). 中-西方音乐对情绪的诱发效应. 心理学报, 48( 7), 757-769. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.00757URL |
[2] | 蔡岳建, 潘孝富, 庄钟春晓 . ( 2007). 音乐的速度与调式对大学生情绪影响的实证研究. 心理科学, 30( 1), 196-198. |
[3] | 马谐, 白学军, 陶云 . ( 2013). 音乐与情绪诱发的机制模型. 心理科学进展, 21( 4), 643-652. |
[4] | Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J . ( 2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98( 20), 11818-11823. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898URLpmid: 11573015 |
[5] | Bogert B., Numminen-Kontti T., Gold B., Sams M., Numminen J., Burunat I., .. Brattico E . ( 2016). Hidden sources of joy, fear, and sadness: Explicit versus implicit neural processing of musical emotions. Neuropsychologia, 89, 393-402. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.07.005URLpmid: 27394152 |
[6] | Brattico E., Alluri V., Bogert B., Jacobsen T., Vartiainen N., Nieminen S. K., & Tervaniemi M . ( 2011). A functional MRI study of happy and sad emotions in music with and without lyrics. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 308. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00308URLpmid: 3227856 |
[7] | Brattico E., Bogert B., Alluri V., Tervaniemi M., Eerola T., & Jacobsen T . ( 2015). It's sad but I like it: The neural dissociation between musical emotions and liking in experts and laypersons. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 676. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00676URLpmid: 4701928 |
[8] | Brattico, E., & Pearce, M. (2013). The neuroaesthetics of music. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, & the Arts, 7( 1), 48-61. doi: 10.1037/a0031624URL |
[9] | Brown S., Martinez M. J., & Parsons L. M . ( 2004). Passive music listening spontaneously engages limbic and paralimbic systems. NeuroReport, 15( 13), 2033-2037. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200409150-00008URLpmid: 15486477 |
[10] | Chamorro-Premuzic T., Fagan P., & Furnham A . ( 2010). Personality and uses of music as predictors of preferences for music consensually classified as happy, sad, complex, and social. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4( 4), 205-213. doi: 10.1037/a0019210URL |
[11] | Eerola, T., & Peltola, H. R . ( 2016). Memorable experiences with sad music—reasons, reactions and mechanisms of three types of experiences. PLoS One, 11( 6), e157444. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157444URLpmid: 27300268 |
[12] | Eerola T., Peltola H. R., & Vuoskoski J. K . ( 2015). Attitudes toward sad music are related to both preferential and contextual strategies. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, & Brain, 25( 2), 116-123. doi: 10.1037/pmu0000096URL |
[13] | Eerola, T., & Vuoskoski, J. K . ( 2011). A comparison of the discrete and dimensional models of emotion in music. Psychology of Music, 39( 1), 18-49. doi: 10.1177/0305735610362821URL |
[14] | Eerola T., Vuoskoski J. K., & Kautiainen H . ( 2016). Being moved by unfamiliar sad music is associated with high empathy. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1176. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01176URLpmid: 27695424 |
[15] | Fritz T., Jentschke S., Gosselin N., Sammler D., Peretz I., Turner R., .. Koelsch S . ( 2009). Universal recognition of three basic emotions in music. Current Biology, 19( 7), 573-576. |
[16] | Gagnon, L., & Peretz, I. (2003). Mode and tempo relative contributions to “happy-sad” judgements in equitone melodies. Cognition & Emotion, 17( 1), 25-40. doi: 10.1080/02699930302279URL |
[17] | Garrido, S., & Schubert, E. (2011). Individual differences in the enjoyment of negative emotion in music: A literature review and experiment. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 28( 3), 279-296. doi: 10.1525/mp.2011.28.3.279URL |
[18] | Garrido, S., & Schubert, E. (2013). Adaptive and maladaptive attraction to negative emotions in music. Musicae Scientiae, 17( 2), 147-166. doi: 10.1177/1029864913478305URL |
[19] | Garrido, S., & Schubert, E. (2015 a). Moody melodies: Do they cheer us up? A study of the effect of sad music on mood. Psychology of Music, 43( 2), 244-261. doi: 10.1177/0305735613501938URL |
[20] | Garrido, S., & Schubert, E. (2015 b). Music and people with tendencies to depression. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 32( 4), 313-321. doi: 10.1525/mp.2015.32.4.313URL |
[21] | Gosselin, N. (2006). Emotional responses to unpleasant music correlates with damage to the parahippocampal cortex. Brain, 129( 10), 2585-2592. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl240URLpmid: 16959817 |
[22] | Green A. C., B?rentsen K. B., St?dkilde-J?rgensen H., Wallentin M., Roepstorff A., & Vuust P . ( 2008). Music in minor activates limbic structures: A relationship with dissonance? NeuroReport, 19( 7), 711-715. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282fd0dd8URLpmid: 18418244 |
[23] | Hogue J. D., Crimmins A. M., & Kahn J. H . ( 2016). "So sad and slow, so why can't I turn off the radio": The effects of gender, depression, and absorption on liking music that induces sadness and music that induces happiness. Psychology of Music, 44( 4), 816-829. doi: 10.1177/0305735615594489URL |
[24] | Hunter P. G., Schellenberg E. G., & Griffith A. T . ( 2011). Misery loves company: Mood-congruent emotional responding to music. Emotion, 11( 5), 1068-1072. doi: 10.1037/a0023749URLpmid: 21639629 |
[25] | Hunter P. G., Schellenberg E. G., & Schimmack U . ( 2010). Feelings and perceptions of happiness and sadness induced by music: Similarities, differences, and mixed emotions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4( 1), 47-56. doi: 10.1037/a0016873URL |
[26] | Huron, D. (2011). Why is sad music pleasurable? A possible role for prolactin. Musicae Scientiae, 15( 2), 146-158. doi: 10.1177/1029864911401171URL |
[27] | Juslin, P. N . ( 2013). From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions: Towards a unified theory of musical emotions. Physics of Life Reviews, 10( 3), 235-266. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2013.05.008URLpmid: 23769678 |
[28] | Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2004). Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: A review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. Journal of New Music Research, 33( 3), 217-238. doi: 10.1080/0929821042000317813URL |
[29] | Juslin, P. N., & V?stfj?ll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31( 5), 559-575. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08006079URLpmid: 18826699 |
[30] | Kawakami A., Furukawa K., Katahira K., & Okanoya K . ( 2013). Sad music induces pleasant emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 311. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00311URLpmid: 23785342 |
[31] | Kawakami, A., & Katahira, K. (2015). Influence of trait empathy on the emotion evoked by sad music and on the preference for it. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1541. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01541URL |
[32] | Khalfa S., Schon D., Anton J. L., & Liegeois-Chauvel C . ( 2005). Brain regions involved in the recognition of happiness and sadness in music. NeuroReport, 16( 18), 1981-1984. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200512190-00002URLpmid: 16317338 |
[33] | Kivy, P. ( 1991). Music alone: Philosophical reflections on the purely musical experience. New York: Cornell University Press. |
[34] | Koelsch, S. (2014). Brain correlates of music-evoked emotions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15( 3), 170-180. doi: 10.1038/nrn3666URLpmid: 24552785 |
[35] | Koelsch S., Fritz T., Cramon D. Y. V., Müller K., & Friederici A. D . ( 2006). Investigating emotion with music: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 27( 3), 239-250. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20180URLpmid: 16078183 |
[36] | Kone?ni, V. J . ( 2005). The aesthetic trinity: Awe, being moved, thrills. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 5, 27-44. |
[37] | Ladinig, O., & Schellenberg, E. G . ( 2012). Liking unfamiliar music: Effects of felt emotion and individual differences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6( 2), 146-154. doi: 10.1037/a0024671URL |
[38] | Lee C. J., Andrade E. B., & Palmer S. E . ( 2013). Interpersonal relationships and preferences for mood- congruency in aesthetic experiences. Journal of Consumer Research, 40( 2), 382-391. doi: 10.1086/670609URL |
[39] | Levinson, J. (1997). Music and negative emotion. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 63( 4), 327-346. |
[40] | Mallik A., Chanda M. L., & Levitin D. J . ( 2017). Anhedonia to music and mu-opioids: Evidence from the administration of naltrexone. Scientific Reports, 7, 41952. doi: 10.1038/srep41952URLpmid: 28176798 |
[41] | Martínez-Molina N., Mas-Herrero E., Rodríguez-Fornells A., Zatorre R. J., & Marco-Pallarés J . ( 2016). Neural correlates of specific musical anhedonia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113( 46), E7337-E7345. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611211113URLpmid: 27799544 |
[42] | Menninghaus W., Wagner V., Hanich J., Wassiliwizky E., Kuehnast M., & Jacobsen T . ( 2015). Towards a psychological construct of being moved. PLoS One, 10( 6), e0128451. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128451URLpmid: 26042816 |
[43] | Menninghaus W., Wagner V., Hanich J., Wassiliwizky E., Jacobsen T., & Koelsch S . ( 2017). The Distancing- Embracing model of the enjoyment of negative emotions in art reception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e347. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X17000309URLpmid: 28215214 |
[44] | Menon, V., & Levitin, D. J . ( 2005). The rewards of music listening: Response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. NeuroImage, 28( 1), 175-184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.053URLpmid: 16023376 |
[45] | Mitterschiffthaler M. T., Fu C. H. Y., Dalton J. A., Andrew C. M., & Williams, S. C. R. (2007). A functional MRI study of happy and sad affective states induced by classical music. Human Brain Mapping, 28( 11), 1150-1162. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20337URLpmid: 17290372 |
[46] | Mori, K., & Iwanaga, M. (2014). Pleasure generated by sadness: Effect of sad lyrics on the emotions induced by happy music. Psychology of Music, 42( 5), 643-652. doi: 10.1177/0305735613483667URL |
[47] | Mori, K., & Iwanaga, M. (2017). Two types of peak emotional responses to music: The psychophysiology of chills and tears. Scientific Reports, 7, 46063. doi: 10.1038/srep46063URLpmid: 5384201 |
[48] | Panksepp, J. (1995). The emotional sources of “chills” induced by music. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13( 2), 171-207. doi: 10.2307/40285693URL |
[49] | Peltola, H. R., & Eerola, T. (2016). Fifty shades of blue: Classification of music-evoked sadness. Musicae Scientiae, 20( 1), 84-102. doi: 10.1177/1029864915611206URL |
[50] | Sachs M. E., Damasio A., & Habibi A . ( 2015). The pleasures of sad music: A systematic review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 404. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00404URLpmid: 4513245 |
[51] | Salimpoor V. N., Benovoy M., Larcher K., Dagher A., & Zatorre R. J . ( 2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nature Neuroscience, 14( 2), 257-262. doi: 10.1038/nn.2726URLpmid: 21217764 |
[52] | Salimpoor V. N., van den Bosch I., Kovacevic N., McIntosh A. R., Dagher A., & Zatorre R. J . ( 2013). Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory cortices predict music reward value. Science, 340( 6129), 216-219. doi: 10.1126/science.1231059URL |
[53] | Salimpoor V. N., Zald D. H., Zatorre R. J., Dagher A., & Mcintosh A. R . ( 2015). Predictions and the brain: How musical sounds become rewarding. T`rends in Cognitive Sciences, 19( 2), 86-91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.001URLpmid: 25534332 |
[54] | Scherer, K. R . ( 2004). Which emotions can be induced by music? What are the underlying mechanisms? And how can we measure them? Journal of New Music Research, 33( 3), 239-251. doi: 10.1080/0929821042000317822URL |
[55] | Scherer, K. R . ( 2005). What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information, 44( 4), 695-729. doi: 10.1177/0539018405058216URL |
[56] | Schubert, E. (1996). Enjoyment of negative emotions in music: An associative network explanation. Psychology of Music, 24( 1), 18-28. doi: 10.1177/0305735696241003URL |
[57] | Schubert, E. (2013). Loved music can make a listener feel negative emotions. Musicae Scientiae, 17( 1), 11-26. doi: 10.1177/1029864912461321URL |
[58] | Schubert, E. (2016). Enjoying sad music: Paradox or parallel processes? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 312. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00312URLpmid: 4920107 |
[59] | Suzuki M., Okamura N., Kawachi Y., Tashiro M., Arao H., Hoshishiba T., … Yanai K . ( 2008). Discrete cortical regions associated with the musical beauty of major and minor chords. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8( 2), 126-131. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.2.126URLpmid: 18589503 |
[60] | Taruffi, L., & Koelsch, S. (2014). The paradox of music- evoked sadness: An online survey. PLoS One, 9( 10), e110490. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110490URLpmid: 4203803 |
[61] | Trost W., Ethofer T., Zentner M., & Vuilleumier P . ( 2012). Mapping aesthetic musical emotions in the brain. Cerebral Cortex, 22( 12), 2769-2783. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr353URLpmid: 22178712 |
[62] | van den Tol, A. J. M . ( 2016). The appeal of sad music: A brief overview of current directions in research on motivations for listening to sad music. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 49, 44-49. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2016.05.008URL |
[63] | van den Tol, A. J. M., & Edwards, J. (2013). Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad. Psychology of Music, 41( 4), 440-465. doi: 10.1177/0305735611430433URL |
[64] | van den Tol, A. J. M., & Edwards, J. (2015). Listening to sad music in adverse situations: How music selection strategies relate to self-regulatory goals, listening effects, and mood enhancement. Psychology of Music, 43( 4), 473-494. doi: 10.1177/0305735613517410URL |
[65] | van den Tol, A. J. M., Edwards J., & Heflick N. A . ( 2016). Sad music as a means for acceptance-based coping. Musicae Scientiae, 20( 1), 68-83. doi: 10.1177/1029864915627844URL |
[66] | Vuoskoski, J. K., & Eerola, T. (2012). Can sad music really make you sad? Indirect measures of affective states induced by music and autobiographical memories. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity & the Arts, 6( 3), 204-213. doi: 10.1037/a0026937URL |
[67] | Vuoskoski, J. K., & Eerola, T. (2017). The pleasure evoked by sad music is mediated by feelings of being moved. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 439. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2017.00439URLpmid: 5481360 |
[68] | Vuoskoski J. K., Thompson W. F., McIlwain D., & Eerola T . ( 2012). Who enjoys listening to sad music and why? Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29( 3), 311-317. doi: 10.1525/mp.2012.29.3.311URL |
[69] | Wassiliwizky E., Jacobsen T., Heinrich J., Schneiderbauer M., & Menninghaus W . ( 2017). Tears falling on goosebumps: Co-occurrence of emotional lacrimation and emotional piloerection indicates a psychophysiological climax in emotional arousal. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 41. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00041URLpmid: 28223946 |
[70] | Weth K., Raab M. H., & Carbon C. C . ( 2015). Investigating emotional responses to self-selected sad music via self- report and automated facial analysis. Musicae Scientiae, 19( 4), 412-432. doi: 10.1177/1029864915606796URL |
[71] | Zatorre, R. J., & Salimpoor, V. N . ( 2013). From perception to pleasure: Music and its neural substrates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110( Supplement 2), 10430-10437. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301228110URLpmid: 23754373 |
[72] | Zentner M., Grandjean D., & Scherer K. R . ( 2008). Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion, 8( 4), 494-521. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494URLpmid: 18729581 |
相关文章 15
[1] | 张辉华. 社会网络视角的团队情绪智力[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1381-1395. |
[2] | 李晓明, 邹是, 高友明. 失望情绪在不作为惯性产生中的作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1396-1401. |
[3] | 张珊珊, 王婧怡, 李昱汝. 情绪自旋及其心理健康功能[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(8): 1430-1437. |
[4] | 曾宪卿, 许冰, 孙博, 叶健彤, 傅世敏. EMMN受偏差-标准刺激对类型和情绪类型影响: 来自元分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1163-1178. |
[5] | 王晓田, 王娜, 何金波. 前瞻性情绪作为社会风险的信息源假说:公共场景下风险决策的情绪及文化机制探讨[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 959-966. |
[6] | 彭坚, 曹兵兵. 追随者主动工作行为的上行影响:内隐追随视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(6): 967-977. |
[7] | 何蔚祺, 李帅霞, 赵东方. 群体面孔情绪感知的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 761-772. |
[8] | 尹俊婷, 王冠, 罗俊龙. 威胁对创造力的影响:认知与情绪双加工路径[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 815-826. |
[9] | 王学思, 李静雅, 王美芳. 父母婚姻冲突对儿童发展的影响及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(5): 875-884. |
[10] | 丁琳洁, 李旭, 尹述飞. 工作记忆中的积极效应:情绪效价与任务相关性的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(4): 652-664. |
[11] | 关旭旭, 王红波. 抑制引起的遗忘及其神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(4): 665-676. |
[12] | 张衍, 王俊秀, 席居哲. 幸灾乐祸的重新审视和互动过程模型的构想[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(3): 505-519. |
[13] | 赵小红, 童薇, 陈桃林, 吴冬梅, 张蕾, 陈正举, 方晓义, 龚启勇, 唐小蓉. 敬畏的心理模型及其认知神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(3): 520-530. |
[14] | 霍鹏辉, 冯成志, 陈庭继. 注视者及观察者因素对注视知觉的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(2): 238-251. |
[15] | 黄挚靖, 李旭. 抑郁症患者工作记忆内情绪刺激加工的特点及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(2): 252-267. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4338