1 西南大学心理学部, 教育部认知与人格重点实验室, 重庆市脑科学协同创新中心西南大学分中心, 重庆 400715
2 北京师范大学心理学部, 认知神经科学与学习国家重点实验室, 北京 100875
3 北京大学心理与认知科学学院, 北京 100871
4 中国科学院心理研究所, 中国科学院心理健康重点实验室, 北京 100101
收稿日期:
2017-10-24出版日期:
2018-11-30发布日期:
2018-10-30基金资助:
* 国家自然科学基金(31400959);国家自然科学基金委员会与德国科学基金会合作研究项目(NSFC61621136008/DFGTRR-169);广东省创新团队珠江团队计划项目(2016ZT06S220);2017年重庆市留学回国人员创业创新支持计划(cx2017049);中央高校基本科研业务费专项资金项目(SWU1809003);中国科学院心理健康重点实验室开放课题基金(KLMH2015G01)To switch or not to switch?Cognitive and neural mechanisms of card switching behavior
SUN Yachen1#, ZHANG Hanqi2,3#, LI Yonghui4, XUE Gui2(), HE Qinghua1,4()1 Faculty of Psychology, Key Lab of Cognition and Personality, Chongqing Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
2 Faculty of Psychology, National Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
3 School of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China
4 Institute of Psychology, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
Received:
2017-10-24Online:
2018-11-30Published:
2018-10-30摘要/Abstract
摘要: 决策是每个人每天都会进行的活动。二择一时, 有人择一而终, 有人变换不定。这种个体差异可能与坚持性人格和认知灵活性有关。本研究旨在探讨这种行为特征背后的认知神经机制, 以便进一步理解决策变换中存在的个体差异。本研究采用了单变量和多变量体素形态学分析的方法分析了350名大学生(其中女性194人, 平均年龄19.97岁)在随机猜牌任务中的换牌频率与大脑灰质体积的相关情况, 探讨了坚持性人格特质和认知灵活性与换牌频率的关系, 并考察了两者在大脑灰质体积与换牌频率中的中介作用。单变量和多变量体素形态学分析结果都表明, 左侧后扣带回、右侧额中回、右侧额极和右侧脑岛区域的灰质体积可以预测被试的换牌频率; 坚持性人格和认知灵活性在其中起到了中介作用。这些结果阐释了换牌频率个体差异的认知机制和神经基础, 对理解为何有的人一味坚持, 而有的人灵活变换背后的原因提供了理论基础, 同时为开发改善非理性决策行为的方法提供了重要的参考价值。
图/表 10
图1随机猜牌任务程序示意图
图1随机猜牌任务程序示意图
图2被试换牌频率的分布直方图
图2被试换牌频率的分布直方图
表1坚持性和认知灵活性的描述统计及其与换牌频率的相关
认知机制 | 平均数 ± 标准差 | 与换牌频率的相关 |
---|---|---|
坚持性 | 116.81 ± 16.61 | r (249) = -0.23, p <.001 |
认知灵活性 | 4.99 ± 2.16 | r (249) = -0.20, p <.001 |
表1坚持性和认知灵活性的描述统计及其与换牌频率的相关
认知机制 | 平均数 ± 标准差 | 与换牌频率的相关 |
---|---|---|
坚持性 | 116.81 ± 16.61 | r (249) = -0.23, p <.001 |
认知灵活性 | 4.99 ± 2.16 | r (249) = -0.20, p <.001 |
表2灰质体积与换牌频率呈正相关的脑区
脑区 | 左/右半球 | 体素数量 | p | MNI坐标x | MNI坐标y | MNI坐标z |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
后扣带回 | 右 | 1788 | 0.01 | 6 | -44 | 22 |
后扣带回 | 左 | 0.03 | -14 | -48 | 34 | |
楔前叶 | 左 | 0.02 | -10 | -64 | 26 | |
楔前叶 | 右 | 0.02 | 10 | -58 | 26 | |
额中回 | 左 | 1522 | 0.01 | -34 | 24 | 32 |
脑岛 | 左 | 0.04 | -36 | 8 | 2 | |
壳核 | 左 | 0.02 | -28 | -2 | -8 | |
额中回 | 右 | 792 | 0.01 | 46 | 10 | 50 |
额极 | 左 | 116 | 0.03 | -34 | 40 | 2 |
表2灰质体积与换牌频率呈正相关的脑区
脑区 | 左/右半球 | 体素数量 | p | MNI坐标x | MNI坐标y | MNI坐标z |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
后扣带回 | 右 | 1788 | 0.01 | 6 | -44 | 22 |
后扣带回 | 左 | 0.03 | -14 | -48 | 34 | |
楔前叶 | 左 | 0.02 | -10 | -64 | 26 | |
楔前叶 | 右 | 0.02 | 10 | -58 | 26 | |
额中回 | 左 | 1522 | 0.01 | -34 | 24 | 32 |
脑岛 | 左 | 0.04 | -36 | 8 | 2 | |
壳核 | 左 | 0.02 | -28 | -2 | -8 | |
额中回 | 右 | 792 | 0.01 | 46 | 10 | 50 |
额极 | 左 | 116 | 0.03 | -34 | 40 | 2 |
图3灰质体积与换牌频率正相关的脑区(R表示被试右侧区域)
图3灰质体积与换牌频率正相关的脑区(R表示被试右侧区域)
表3灰质体积与换牌频率呈负相关的脑区
脑区 | 大脑半球 | 体素数量 | p | MNI坐标x | MNI坐标y | MNI坐标z |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
颞上回 | 右 | 1541 | 0.05 | 66 | -16 | 6 |
脑岛 | 右 | 0.04 | 34 | -26 | 12 | |
颞叶 | 右 | 998 | 0.01 | 60 | 12 | -26 |
内侧颞叶 | 右 | 379 | 0.02 | 18 | -10 | -22 |
颞叶 | 左 | 280 | 0.02 | -56 | 16 | 30 |
表3灰质体积与换牌频率呈负相关的脑区
脑区 | 大脑半球 | 体素数量 | p | MNI坐标x | MNI坐标y | MNI坐标z |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
颞上回 | 右 | 1541 | 0.05 | 66 | -16 | 6 |
脑岛 | 右 | 0.04 | 34 | -26 | 12 | |
颞叶 | 右 | 998 | 0.01 | 60 | 12 | -26 |
内侧颞叶 | 右 | 379 | 0.02 | 18 | -10 | -22 |
颞叶 | 左 | 280 | 0.02 | -56 | 16 | 30 |
图4灰质体积与换牌频率负相关的脑区(R表示被试右侧区域)
图4灰质体积与换牌频率负相关的脑区(R表示被试右侧区域)
表4可以预测换牌频率的脑区
脑区 | 大脑半球 | 体素数量 | 预测准确性 | MNI坐标x | MNI坐标y | MNI坐标z |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
额中回 | 左 | 222 | 0.18 | -38 | 22 | 32 |
额中回 | 右 | 128 | 0.16 | 48 | 8 | 48 |
脑岛 | 右 | 75 | 0.16 | 30 | -26 | 20 |
额极 | 右 | 18 | 0.16 | 16 | 50 | 46 |
后扣带回 | 左 | 10 | 0.15 | -2 | -44 | 24 |
表4可以预测换牌频率的脑区
脑区 | 大脑半球 | 体素数量 | 预测准确性 | MNI坐标x | MNI坐标y | MNI坐标z |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
额中回 | 左 | 222 | 0.18 | -38 | 22 | 32 |
额中回 | 右 | 128 | 0.16 | 48 | 8 | 48 |
脑岛 | 右 | 75 | 0.16 | 30 | -26 | 20 |
额极 | 右 | 18 | 0.16 | 16 | 50 | 46 |
后扣带回 | 左 | 10 | 0.15 | -2 | -44 | 24 |
图5可以预测换牌频率的脑区(R表示被试右侧区域)
图5可以预测换牌频率的脑区(R表示被试右侧区域)
图6中介效应模型
图6中介效应模型
参考文献 44
1 | Aron A.R . ( 2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: Developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry, 69( 12), e55-e68. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024 |
2 | Baliki M. N., Geha P. Y., & Apkarian A. V . ( 2009). Parsing pain perception between nociceptive representation and magnitude estimation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101( 2), 875-87. |
3 | Bechara A., Damasio H., Tranel D., & Damasio A. R . ( 1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275( 5304), 1293. |
4 | Bechara A., & Damasio A.R . ( 2005). The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games and Economic Behavior, 52( 2), 336-372. doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2004.06.010URL |
5 | , , Bereczkei T., &Czibor A. , ( 2014). Personality and situational factors differently influence high Mach and low Mach persons’ decisions in a social dilemma game. Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 168-173. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.02.035URL |
6 | Clark L., Bechara A., Damasio H., Aitken M. R. F., Sahakian B. J., & Robbins T. W . ( 2008). Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making. Brain, 131( 5), 1311-1322. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn066URLpmid: 2367692 |
7 | Cloninger C. R., Svrakic D. M., & Przybeck T. R . ( 1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of general psychiatry, 50( 12), 975-990. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008.URLpmid: 8250684 |
8 | Daw N. D., O’doherty J. P., Dayan P., Seymour B., & Dolan R. J . ( 2006). Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature, 441( 7095), 876-879. |
9 | De Martino B., Kumaran D., Seymour B., & Dolan R. J . ( 2006). Frames, Biases, and Rational Decision-Making in the Human Brain. Science, 313( 5787), 684-687. doi: 10.1126/science.1128356URLpmid: 16888142 |
10 | Dong X., Du X., & Qi B . ( 2016). Conceptual knowledge influences decision making differently in individuals with high or low cognitive flexibility: An ERP study. Plos One, 11( 8), e0158875. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158875URLpmid: 27479484 |
11 | Feather N.T . ( 1962). The study of persistence. Psychological Bulletin, 59( 2), 94-115. |
12 | Gardini S., Cloninger C. R., & Venneri A . ( 2009). Individual differences in personality traits reflect structural variance in specific brain regions. Brain Research Bulletin, 79( 5), 265-270. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.03.005URLpmid: 19480986 |
13 | Green C. S., Benson C., Kersten D., & Schrater P . ( 2010). Alterations in choice behavior by manipulations of world model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107( 37), 16401-16406. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001709107URLpmid: 20805507 |
14 | Hakamata Y., Lwase M., Lwata H., Kobayashi T., Tamaki T., Nishio M., … Lnada T . ( 2006). Regional brain cerebral glucose metabolism and temperament: A positron emission tomography study. Neuroscience Letters, 396( 1), 33-37. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.11.017URLpmid: 16356648 |
15 | Hanke M., Halchenko Y. O., Sederberg P. B., Hanson S. J., Haxby J. V., & Pollmann S . ( 2009). PyMVPA: A python toolbox for multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI data. Neuroinformatics, 7( 1), 37-53. doi: 10.1007/s12021-008-9041-yURLpmid: 2664559 |
16 | Heath A. C., Cloninger C. R., & Martin N. G . ( 1994). Testing a model for the genetic structure of personality: A comparison of the personality systems of Cloninger and Eysenck. Journal of personality and social psychology, 66( 4), 762-775. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.762URLpmid: 8189351 |
17 | Heaton R., Chelune G., Talley J., Kay G., & Curtiss G . ( 1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual: Revised and expanded. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. |
18 | Ishii H., Ohara S., Tobler P. N., Tsutsui K., & Iijima T . ( 2012). Inactivating anterior insular cortex reduces risk taking. Journal of Neuroscience the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 32( 45), 16031-16039. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2278-12.2012URLpmid: 23136439 |
19 | Jimura K., & Poldrack R.A . ( 2012). Analyses of regional- average activation and multivoxel pattern information tell complementary stories. Neuropsychologia, 50( 4), 544-552. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.007URLpmid: 22100534 |
20 | Jung Y. C., Ku J., Namkoong K., Lee W., Kim S. I., & Kim J. J . ( 2010). Human orbitofrontal-striatum functional connectivity modulates behavioral persistence. Neuroreport, 21( 7), 502-6. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283383482URLpmid: 20357688 |
21 | Kriegeskorte N., Goebel R., & Bandettini P . ( 2006). Information-based functional brain mapping. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103( 10), 3863-3868. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600244103URLpmid: 16537458 |
22 | &Macaskill A.C, Hackenberg T.D . ( 2012). The sunk cost effect with pigeons: Some determinants of decisions about persistence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 97( 1), 85-100. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2012.97-85URLpmid: 22287806 |
23 | Marsh R., Zhu H., Schultz R. T., Quackenbush G., Royal J., Wang Z., Skudlarski P., & Peterson B. S . ( 2006). A developmental fMRI study of self-regulatory control in Tourette's syndrome. Human Brain Mapping, 27(11), 848-863. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20225URLpmid: 2292452 |
24 | Li M.H., & Bai X.J . ( 2005). The research progress of the development of cognitive flexibility in executive function. Psychological exploration, 25(2), 35-38. |
25 | [ 李美华, 白学军 . ( 2005). 执行功能中认知灵活性发展的研究进展. 心理学探新, 25( 2), 35-38.] |
26 | Li Y. H., Chen R., & He Q. H . ( 2018). The application of functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy in understanding the brain mechanism of social interaction. Journal of Psychological Science, 41( 2), 305-311. |
27 | [ 李玉华, 陈睿, 何清华 . ( 2018). 功能性近红外光谱技术在社会互动脑机制研究中的应用. 心理科学, 41( 2), 305-311.] |
28 | Liu Z.N . ( 1999). Clinical application of the Wisconsin card classification test. Foreign Medical Sciences (Section of Psychiatry), 26( 1), 6-9. |
29 | [ 刘哲宁 . ( 1999). Wisconsin 卡片分类测验的临床运用. 国外医学: 精神病学分册, 26( 1), 6-9.] |
30 | Ogino Y., Nemoto H., Inui K., Saito S., Kakigi Ryusuke., & Goto Fumio . ( 2007). Inner experience of pain: Imagination of pain while viewing images showing painful events forms subjective pain representation in human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 17( 5), 1139-1146. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl023URLpmid: 16855007 |
31 | Pittenger D.J . ( 2002). The two paradigms of persistence. Genetic Social & General Psychology Monographs, 128( 3), 237. doi: 10.1007/s00787-002-0272-4URLpmid: 12401034 |
32 | Shu C., Wang G. H., Wang H. L., & Wang X. P . ( 2009). The role of anterior cingle in the Wisconsin card classification test. Chinese Journal of Behavioral Medicine and Brain Science, 18( 12), 1083-1084. |
33 | [ 舒畅, 王高华, 王惠玲, 王晓萍 . ( 2009). 前扣带回在威斯康星卡片分类测验操作中的作用. 中华行为医学与脑科学杂志, 18( 12), 1083-1084.] doi: 10.3760/cma.j.issn.1674-6554.2009.12.010URL |
34 | Shu C., Wang G. H., Wang H. L., Wu G. Y.., Huang X., Sun J. M., & Li Q. Y . ( 2004). Preliminary study on brain activity patterns in WCST operation. Chinese Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 30( 3), 223-225. |
35 | [ 舒畅, 王高华, 王惠玲, 吴光耀, 黄雄, 孙骏谟, 李秋英 . ( 2004). WCST 操作时脑活动模式的初步研究. 中国神经精神疾病杂志, 30( 3), 223-225.] doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1002-0152.2004.03.022URL |
36 | Van Schuerbeek P., Baeken C., De Raedt R., De Mey J., & Luypaert R . ( 2011). Individual differences in local gray and white matter volumes reflect differences in temperament and character: A voxel-based morphometry study in healthy young females. Brain Research, 1371( 2), 32-42. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.073URLpmid: 21126511 |
37 | Wang Y., Ma N., He X. S., Li N., Wei Z. D., Yang L., … &. Zhang X.C . ( 2017). Neural substrates of updating the prediction through prediction error during decision making. Neuroimage, 157, 1-12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.041URLpmid: 28536046 |
38 | Watanabe J., Sugiura M., Sato K., Sato Y., Maeda Y., Matsue Y., … Kawashima R . ( 2002). The human prefrontal and parietal association cortices are involved in NO-GO performances: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 17( 3), 1207-1216. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1198URLpmid: 12414261 |
39 | Wicker B., Keysers C., Plailly J., Royet J-P., Gallese V., & Rizzolatti G . ( 2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40( 3), 655-664. |
40 | Xue G., He Q. H., Lei X. M., Chen C. H., Liu Y. Y., Chen C. S., … Bechara A . ( 2012). The gambler’s fallacy is associated with weak affective decision making but strong cognitive ability. Plos One, 7( 10), e47019. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047019URLpmid: 23071701 |
41 | Xue G., Juan C-H., Chang C-F., Lu Z-L., & Dong Q . ( 2012). Lateral prefrontal cortex contributes to maladaptive decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109( 12), 4401-4406. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1111927109URL |
42 | Xue G., Lu Z. L., Levin I. P., & Bechara A . ( 2010). The impact of prior risk experiences on subsequent risky decision-making: The role of the insula. Neurolmage, 50( 2), 709-716. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.097URLpmid: 20045470 |
43 | Xue G., Lu Z. L., Levin I. P., Weller J. A., Li X. R., & Bechara Antoine . ( 2009). Functional Dissociations of Risk and Reward Processing in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 19( 5), 1019-1027. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn147URLpmid: 2665154 |
44 | Zheng D., Oka T., Bokura H., & Yamaguchi S . ( 2008). The key locus of common response inhibition network for no-go and stop signals. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20( 8), 1434-1442. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20100URLpmid: 18303978 |
相关文章 15
[1] | 熊承清, 许佳颖, 马丹阳, 刘永芳. 囚徒困境博弈中对手面部表情对合作行为的影响及其作用机制[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(8): 919-932. |
[2] | 宋锡妍, 程亚华, 谢周秀甜, 龚楠焰, 刘雷. 愤怒情绪对延迟折扣的影响:确定感和控制感的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(5): 456-468. |
[3] | 尚雪松, 陈卓, 陆静怡. 帮忙失败后我会被差评吗?好心帮倒忙中的预测偏差[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(3): 291-305. |
[4] | 张银玲, 虞祯, 买晓琴. 社会价值取向对自我-他人风险决策的影响及其机制[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(7): 895-908. |
[5] | 王天鸿, 陈宇琦, 陆静怡. 差距知觉的泛化效应:我和你之间的差距有多大?[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(11): 1327-1339. |
[6] | 王盼盼,何嘉梅. 情景预见对跨期决策的影响机制[J]. 心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 38-54. |
[7] | 辛潇洋,李瑛,毕研玲,晏碧华. 一种齐当别思想下的量子决策模型:对囚徒困境中的分离效应的解释[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(6): 724-733. |
[8] | 姚卿, 陈荣. 质量还是价格?折衷产品缺货情境下的消费者选择[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(5): 625-636. |
[9] | 王晓田. 如何用行为经济学应对不确定性:拓展有效助推的范围[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(4): 407-414. |
[10] | 刘隽,张震,孙彦,韩布新,陆勤,刘萍萍. 接受还是拒绝? 反应模式助推基础课与拓展课均衡选择[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(4): 437-449. |
[11] | 杨玲,王斌强,耿银凤,姚东伟,曹华,张建勋,许琼英. 虚拟和真实金钱奖赏幅度对海洛因戒断者风险决策的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(4): 507-516. |
[12] | 周蕾,李爱梅,张磊,李纾,梁竹苑. 风险决策和跨期决策的过程比较:以确定效应和即刻效应为例[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(3): 337-352. |
[13] | 倪旭东,季百乐. 如何消除子团队的消极作用——子团队成员交换的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(2): 259-268. |
[14] | 王鹏,王晓田,高娟,黎夏岚,徐静. 适应性时间管理:死亡意识对时间知觉和跨期决策的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(12): 1341-1350. |
[15] | 徐岚,陈全,崔楠,陆凯丽. 享受当下, 还是留待未来?——时间观对跨期决策的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019, 51(1): 96-105. |
PDF全文下载地址:
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4330