删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

动作的主动控制感与因果关系的主动控制感:主动动作时间压缩效应的产生机制

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

吴迪1,2, 顾晶金1,2, 李明3, 张淼1,2, 张明4,5(), 赵科1,2, 傅小兰1,2
1 中国科学院心理研究所, 脑与认知科学国家重点实验室, 北京 100101
2 中国科学院大学心理学系, 北京 100049
3 吉首大学, 湖南 吉首 416000
4 中国科学院心理研究所心理健康重点实验室, 北京 100101
5 大连医科大学心理学系, 辽宁 大连 116044
收稿日期:2018-03-31出版日期:2019-05-15发布日期:2019-03-20
通讯作者:张明E-mail:zhaok@psych.ac.cn

基金资助:* 中国国家自然科学基金(31400876);中国国家自然科学基金(61632004);中国国家自然科学基金(61621136008);德国研究基金项目(DFG TRR-169)

Sense of agency based on action and causation: The mechanism of intentional binding effect for voluntary action

WU Di1,2, GU Jingjin1,2, LI Ming3, ZHANG Miao1,2, ZHANG Ming4,5(), ZHAO Ke1,2, FU Xiaolan1,2
1 State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
2 Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
3 Jishou University, Jishou 416000, China
4 Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
5 Department of Psychology, Dalian Medical University, Dalian, China, Beijing 116044, China
Received:2018-03-31Online:2019-05-15Published:2019-03-20
Contact:ZHANG Ming E-mail:zhaok@psych.ac.cn






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 主动动作的时间压缩效应指主动动作到动作结果之间存在时间上的主观压缩。基于动作的主动控制感和因果关系的主动控制感是该效应的两种理论解释。本文首先分别介绍动作的主动控制感和因果关系的主动控制感, 并从行为和脑机制角度提供相应的证据解释时间压缩效应的机制。同时, 本文提出了在主动动作时间压缩效应中动作的主动控制感与因果关系的主动控制感之间可能存在的序列加工和平行加工的关系。



图1Haggard等人(2002)研究结果示意图。当要求被试主动按键时, 被试报告的动作发生时刻点靠后(15 ms), 而报告的声音刺激出现的时刻点提前(46 ms)。
图1Haggard等人(2002)研究结果示意图。当要求被试主动按键时, 被试报告的动作发生时刻点靠后(15 ms), 而报告的声音刺激出现的时刻点提前(46 ms)。



图2主动动作时间压缩效应的两阶段序列加工示意图
图2主动动作时间压缩效应的两阶段序列加工示意图







[1] Band G. P. H., van Steenbergen H., Ridderinkhof K. R., Falkenstein M., & Hommel B . ( 2009). Action-effect negativity: Irrelevant action effects are monitored like relevant feedback. Biological Psychology, 82( 3), 211-218. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.011
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.011URL
[2] Bastian, A. J . ( 2006). Learning to predict the future: The cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16( 6), 645-649. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.08.016
doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.08.016URL
[3] Blakemore S. J., Frith C. D., & Wolpert D. M . ( 1999a). Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11( 5), 551-559.
doi: 10.1162/089892999563607URL
[4] Blakemore S. J., Frith C. D., & Wolpert D. M . ( 2001). The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action. Neuroreport, 12( 9), 1879-1884. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200107030-00023
doi: 10.1097/00001756-200107030-00023URL
[5] Blakemore S. J., Wolpert D. M., & Frith C. D . ( 1998). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 1( 7), 635-640. doi: 10.1038/2870
[6] Blakemore S. J., Wolpert D. M., & Frith C. D . ( 1999b). The cerebellum contributes to somatosensory cortical activity during self-produced tactile stimulation. Neuroimage, 10( 4), 448-459. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0478
doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0478URL
[7] Buehner, M. J . ( 2012). Understanding the past, predicting the future: Causation, not intentional action, is the root of temporal binding. Psychological Science, 23( 12), 1490-1497. doi: 10.1177/0956797612444612
doi: 10.1177/0956797612444612URL
[8] Buehner, M. J., & Humphreys, G. R . ( 2009). Causal binding of actions to their effects. Psychological Science, 20( 10), 1221-1228. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02435.x
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02435.xURL
[9] Capozzi F., Becchio C., Garbarini F., Savazzi S., & Pia L . ( 2016). Temporal perception in joint action: This is MY action. Consciousness and Cognition, 40, 26-33. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.004
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.004URL
[10] Caspar E. A., Christensen J. F., Cleeremans A., & Haggard P . ( 2016). Coercion changes the sense of agency in the human brain. Current Biology, 26( 5), 585-592. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.067
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.067URL
[11] Cavazzana A., Begliomini C., & Bisiacchi P. S . ( 2014). Intentional binding effect in children: Insights from a new paradigm. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 651. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00651
[12] Desantis A., Roussel C., & Waszak F . ( 2011). On the influence of causal beliefs on the feeling of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 20( 4), 1211-1220. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.012
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.012URL
[13] Eagleman, D. M., & Holcombe A.O, . ( 2002). Causality and the perception of time. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6( 8), 323-325.
doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01945-9URL
[14] Elsner B., Hommel B., Mentschel C., Drzezga A., Prinz W., Conrad B., & Siebner H . ( 2002). Linking actions and their perceivable consequences in the human brain. Neuroimage, 17( 1), 364-372. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1162
doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1162URL
[15] Engbert, K. & Wohlschlager A. , ( 2007). Intentions and expectations in temporal binding. Consciousness and Cognition, 16( 2), 255-264. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.09.010
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.09.010URL
[16] Eppinger B., Kray J., Mock B., & Mecklinger A . ( 2008). Better or worse than expected? Aging, learning, and the ERN. Neuropsychologia, 46( 2), 521-539. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.001
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.001URL
[17] Haggard, P. ( 2017). Sense of agency in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18( 4), 196-207.
[18] Haggard P., Clark S., & Kalogeras J . ( 2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5( 4), 382-385. doi: 10.1038/nn827
[19] Hume D., . ( 1888). Treatise of human nature. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press. (Original work published 1777)
[20] Humphreys, G. R., & Buehner M.J, . ( 2009). Magnitude estimation reveals temporal binding at super-second intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35( 5), 1542-1549. doi: 10.1037/a0014492
doi: 10.1037/a0014492URL
[21] James W., .( 1890). The Principles of Psychology: Vol. 2 . New York: Dover.
[22] Jo H. G., Wittmann M., Hinterberger T., & Schmidt S . ( 2014). The readiness potential reflects intentional binding. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 421. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00421
[23] Kawabe T., Roseboom W., & Nishida S . ( 2013). The sense of agency is action-effect causality perception based on cross-modal grouping. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280( 1763). doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0991
[24] Melcher T., Weidema M., Eenshuistra R. M., Hommel B., & Gruber O . ( 2008). The neural substrate of the ideomotor principle: An event-related fMRI analysis. Neuroimage, 39( 3), 1274-1288. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.049
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.049URL
[25] Moore J. & Haggard P. , ( 2008). Awareness of action: Inference and prediction. Consciousness and Cognition, 17( 1), 136-144. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.12.004
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.12.004URL
[26] Moore J. W., Cambridge V. C., Morgan H., Giorlando F., Adapa R., & Fletcher P. C . ( 2013). Time, action and psychosis: Using subjective time to investigate the effects of ketamine on sense of agency. Neuropsychologia, 51( 2), 377-384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.005
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.005URL
[27] Moore, J. W., & Obhi S.S . ( 2012). Intentional binding and the sense of agency: A review. Consciousness and Cognition, 21( 1), 546-561. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.12.002
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.12.002URL
[28] Pacherie E., . (2008). The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework. Cognition, 107( 1), 179-217. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.003
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.003URL
[29] Ruess M., Thomaschke R., & Kiesel A . ( 2018). Intentional binding of visual effects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80( 3), 713-722. doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1479-2
[30] Scholl, B. J., & Tremoulet P.D, . ( 2000). Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4( 8), 299-309. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01506-0
doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01506-0URL
[31] Sidarus N. & Haggard P. ,( 2016). Difficult action decisions reduce the sense of agency: A study using the Eriksen flanker task. Acta Psychologica, 166, 1-11. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.03.003
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.03.003URL
[32] Synofzik M., Vosgerau G., & Newen A . ( 2008). Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agency. Consciousness and cognition, 17( 1), 219-239.
[33] Ticini L. F., Schutz-Bosbach S., Weiss C., Casile A., & Waszak F . ( 2012). When sounds become actions: Higher- order representation of newly learned action sounds in the human motor system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24( 2), 464-474. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00134
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00134URL
[34] Voss M., Chambon V., Wenke D., Kuhn S., & Haggard P . ( 2017). In and out of control: Brain mechanisms linking fluency of action selection to self-agency in patients with schizophrenia. Brain, 140( 8), 2226-2239. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx136
doi: 10.1093/brain/awx136URL
[35] Voss M., Moore J., Hauser M., Gallinat J., Heinz A., & Haggard P . ( 2010). Altered awareness of action in schizophrenia: A specific deficit in predicting action consequences. Brain, 133( 10), 3104-3112. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq152
doi: 10.1093/brain/awq152URL
[36] Waszak F., Cardoso-Leite P., & Hughes G . ( 2012). Action effect anticipation: Neurophysiological basis and functional consequences. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36( 2), 943-959. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.004
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.004URL
[37] Wen W., Yamashita A., & Asama H . ( 2015). The influence of action-outcome delay and arousal on sense of agency and the intentional binding effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 87-95. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.004
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.004URL
[38] Wolpert, D. M . ( 1997). Computational approaches to motor control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1( 6), 209-216. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01070-X
doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01070-XURL
[39] Woods A. J., Hamilton R. H., Kranjec A., Minhaus P., Bikson M., Yu J., & Chatterjee A . ( 2014). Space, time, and causality in the human brain. Neuroimage, 92, 285-297. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.02.015
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.02.015URL
[40] Yabe Y., & Goodale M.A, . ( 2015). Time flies when we intend to act: Temporal distortion in a go/no-go task. Journal of Neuroscience, 35( 12), 5023-5029. doi: 10.1523/Jneurosci.4386-14.2015
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4386-14.2015URL
[41] Zhao K., Chen Y. H., Yan W. J., & Fu X . ( 2013). To bind or not to bind? Different temporal binding effects from voluntary pressing and releasing actions. PLoS One, 8( 5), e64819. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064819
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064819URL
[42] Zhao K., Gu R., Wang L., Xiao P., Chen Y. H., Liang J., .. Fu X. L . ( 2014). Voluntary pressing and releasing actions induce different senses of time: Evidence from event-related brain responses. Scientific Reports, 4, 6047. doi: 10.1038/srep06047
[43] Zhao K., Hu L., Qu F. B., Cui Q., Piao Q. H., Xu H., .. Fu X. L . ( 2016). Voluntary action and tactile sensory feedback in the intentional binding effect. Experimental Brain Research. 234( 8), 2283-2292. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4633-5




[1]张淼, 吴迪, 李明, 凌懿白, 张明, 赵科. 主动控制感的测量及认知神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(10): 1787-1793.
[2]姜永志. 西方心理学理论分类体系的反思与建构[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(2): 348-356.
[3]王墨耘. 单一因果关系因果力判断研究述评[J]. 心理科学进展, 2004, 12(1): 18-27.
[4]冷英;莫雷. 隐含因果关系影响代词解决的研究进展[J]. 心理科学进展, 2002, 10(1): 36-41.
[5]王亚同. 课文理解的因果关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 1996, 4(4): 39-44.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=4677
相关话题/心理 控制 科学 序列 北京