删除或更新信息,请邮件至freekaoyan#163.com(#换成@)

公共政策情境中行为习惯的形成机制与培养策略

本站小编 Free考研考试/2022-01-01

杜立婷1(), 李东进2
1天津工业大学人文学院, 天津 300387
2南开大学商学院, 天津 300071
收稿日期:2019-09-17出版日期:2020-07-15发布日期:2020-05-21
通讯作者:杜立婷E-mail:master_dlt@hotmail.com

基金资助:* 国家自然科学基金项目(71702127);国家自然科学基金项目(71772092);国家自然科学基金重点项目(71832005);天津市哲学社会科学规划项目(TJGL16-008Q)

Mechanisms and strategies for cultivating behavioral habits within the field of public policy

DU Liting1(), LI Dongjin2
1School of Humanities, Tiangong University, Tianjin 300387, China
2Business School, Nankai University, Tianjin 300071, China
Received:2019-09-17Online:2020-07-15Published:2020-05-21
Contact:DU Liting E-mail:master_dlt@hotmail.com






摘要/Abstract


摘要: 引导社会成员重塑日常良好行为习惯是增强公共政策有效性的重要路径和切入点, 洞悉并把握行为习惯的形成机制对具体培养策略的制定和实施大有裨益。行为习惯是指通过高频重复的奖励习得, 在记忆中形成的情境与反应之间的内隐联结。作为一种特殊的内隐记忆系统, 行为习惯具有自动化、情境线索依赖性、对当前奖励低敏感性等特征。行为习惯的形成机制包含两个独立且共发的习得机制, 即联结习得机制和奖励习得机制。在日常良好行为习惯的形成初期, 目标引导着个体对行为价值的习得。在培养社会成员日常良好行为习惯的过程中, 应将情境线索的稳定性、高频行为的低认知重复以及奖励的合理设置, 作为公共政策助推社会成员养成日常良好行为习惯的三大核心策略。未来的研究方向应着力于加强习惯研究的学科视阈融合, 更新行为习惯领域的研究范式, 重置公共设施以提升有益行为的易达性, 以及引入商业模式以共促社会成员日常良好行为习惯的重塑。


[1] 杜立婷. (2015). 购买习惯与态度忠诚的混淆与辨析. 理论与现代化, (3), 95-101.
[2] 杜立婷, 李东进. (2017). 购买习惯对新产品选择行为的双重影响模型及实证检验. 管理学报, 14(7), 1070-1078.
[3] 李东进, 张宇东. (2018). 消费领域的量化自我:研究述评与展望. 外国经济与管理, 40(1), 3-17.
[4] 吕小康, 武迪, 隋晓阳, 汪新建, 程婕婷. (2018). 从“理性人”到“行为人”:公共政策研究的行为科学转向. 心理科学进展, 26(12), 2249-2259.
[5] Anderson, B. A. (2016). The attention habit: How reward learning shapes attentional selection. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1369(1), 24-39.
URLpmid: 26595376
[6] Austin, C. G., & Kwapisz, A. (2017). The road to unintended consequences is paved with motivational apps. The Journal of Consumer Affairs. 51(2), 463-477.
[7] Beshears, J., Lee, H. N., Milkman, K. L., & Mislavsky, R. (2017). Creating exercise habits using incentives: The tradeoff between flexibility and routinization. https://pdfs.semanticscholar. org/a3f9/fd231a481b088cdb0c351d8672fdbd3cda33.pdf
[8] Bless, H., & Burger, A. M. (2016). A closer look at social psychologists’ silver bullet: Inevitable and evitable side effects of the experimental approach. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(2), 296-308.
URLpmid: 26993280
[9] Bonini, N., Hadjichristidis, C., & Graffeo, M. (2018). 绿色助推. 心理学报, 50(8), 814-826.
[10] Botvinick, M. M., & Weinstein, A. (2014). Model-based hierarchical reinforcement learning and human action control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, 369(1655), 20130480.
[11] Brooks, T. L., Leventhal, H., Wolf, M. S., O’Conor, R., Morillo, J., Martynenko, M., ... Federman, A. D. (2014). Strategies used by older adults with asthma for adherence to inhaled corticosteroids. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 29(11), 1506-1512.
URLpmid: 25092003
[12] Burton, A. C., Nakamura, K., & Roesch, M. R. (2015). From ventral-medial to dorsal-lateral striatum: Neural correlates of reward-guided decision-making. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 117, 51-59.
doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.05.003URLpmid: 24858182
[13] Carden, L., Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2017). Pascoe A. Incentives and consumer’s belief in control: Challenges for habit performance. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 2(3), 279-290.
[14] Cooper, R. P., Ruh, N., & Mareschal, D. (2014). The goal circuit model: A hierarchical multi-route model of the acquisition and control of routine sequential action in humans. Cognitive Science, 38(2), 244-274.
URLpmid: 23941131
[15] Daw, N. D., Niv, Y., & Dayan, P. (2005). Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nature Neuroscience, 8(12), 1704-1711.
URLpmid: 16286932
[16] Ersche, K. D., Gillan, C. M., Jones, P. S., Williams, G. B., Ward, L. H., Luijten, M., ... Robbins, T. W. (2016). Carrots and sticks fail to change behavior in cocaine addiction . Science, 352(6292), 1468-1471.
URLpmid: 27313048
[17] Etkin, J. (2015). The hidden cost of personal quantification. Journal of Consumer Research, 42(6), 967-984.
[18] FitzGerald, H. B., Dolan, R. J., Friston, K. J. (2014). Model averaging, optimal inference, and habit formation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 457.
URLpmid: 25018724
[19] Friedel, E., Koch, S. P., Wendt, J., Heinz, A., Deserno, L., & Schlagenhauf, F. (2014). Devaluation and sequential decisions: Linking goal-directed and model-based behavior. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 587.
URLpmid: 25136310
[20] Fujii, N., & Graybiel, A. M. (2003). Representation of action sequence boundaries by macaque prefrontal cortical neurons. Science, 301(5637), 1246-1249.
URLpmid: 12947203
[21] Galla, B. M., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(3), 508-525.
[22] Gardner, B. (2015). A review and analysis of the use of “habit” in understanding, predicting and influencing health-related behaviour. Health Psychology Review, 9(3), 277-295.
URLpmid: 25207647
[23] Gershman, S. J., Markman, A. B., & Otto, A. R. (2014). Retrospective revaluation in sequential decision making: A tale of two systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 182-94
[24] Gillan, C. M., Otto, A. R., Phelps, E. A., & Daw, N. D. (2015). Model-based learning protects against forming habits. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, (3),523-536.
[25] Gl?veanu, V. (2012). Habitual Creativity: Revising habit, reconceptualizing creativity. Review of General Psychology, 16(1), 78-92.
[26] Gneezy, U., Meier, S., & Rey-Biel, P. (2011). When and why incentives (don’t) work to modify behavior. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 25(4), 191-209.
[27] Judah, G., Gardner, B., & Aunger, R. (2013). Forming a flossing habit: An exploratory study of the psychological determinants of habit formation. British Journal of Health Psychology, 18(2), 338-353.
[28] Labrecque, J. S., Wood, W., Neal, D. T., & Harrington, N. (2017). Habit slips: When consumers unintentionally resist new products. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 45(1), 119-133.
[29] Lin, P.-Y., Wood, W., & Monterosso, J. (2016). Healthy eating habits protect against temptations. Appetite, 103, 432-440.
[30] Lourenc?o, J. S., Ciriolo, E., Almeida, S. R., & Troussard, X. (2016). Behavioural insights applied to policy: European Report, Report No. EUR 27726 EN.
[31] Mantzari, E., Vogt, F., Shemilt, I., Wei, Y. H., Higgins, J. P. T., & Marteau, T. M. (2015). Personal financial incentives for changing habitual health-related behaviors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Preventive Medicine, 75, 75-85.
doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.03.001URLpmid: 25843244
[32] Moore, L. V., & Thompson, F. E. (2015). Adults meeting fruit and vegetable intake recommendations-United States, 2013. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 64(26), 709-713.
[33] Neal, D. T., Vujcic, J. Hernandez, O., & Wood, W. (2015). Creating hand-washing habits: Six principles for creating disruptive and sticky behavior change for hand washing with soap. Unpublished manuscript, Catalyst Behavioral Science, Miami, FL.
[34] Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S., & Lally, P. (2011). How do habits guide behavior? Perceived and actual triggers of habits in daily life. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 492-498.
[35] Orbell, S., & Verplanken, B. (2010). The automatic component of habit in health behavior: Habit as cue-contingent automaticity. Health Psychology, 29(4), 374-383.
[36] Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F., & Chersi, F. (2013). The mixed instrumental controller: Using value of information to combine habitual choice and mental simulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 92.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00092URLpmid: 23459512
[37] Rogers, T., Milkman, K. L., John, L. K., & Norton, M. I. (2015). Beyond good intentions: Prompting people to make plans improves follow-through on important tasks. Behavioral Science & Policy, 1(2), 33-41.
[38] Salmon, S. J., Fennis, B. M., de Ridder, D. T. D., Adriaanse, M. A., & de Vet, E. (2014). Health on impulse: When low self-control promotes healthy food choices. Health Psychology, 33(2), 103-109.
doi: 10.1037/a0031785URLpmid: 23477580
[39] Shen. L., X., Hsee, C. K., & Talloen, J. (2019). The fun and function of uncertainty: Uncertain incentives reinforce repetition decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 46(1), 69-81.
[40] Smith, K. S., & Graybiel, A. M. (2016). Habit formation. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 18(1), 33-43.
[41] Stawarz, K., Cox, A. L., & Blandford, A. (2015). Beyond self-tracking and reminders: Designing smartphone apps that support habit formation. Human Factors in Computing Systems, 2653-2662.
[42] Taatgen, N. A., Huss, D., Dickison, D., & Anderson, J. R. (2008). The acquisition of robust and flexible cognitive skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 137(3), 548-565.
[43] Tappe, K., Tarves, E., Oltarzewski, J., & Frum, D. (2013). Habit formation among regular exercisers at fitness centers: an exploratory study. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 10(4), 607-613.
URLpmid: 22976286
[44] Thaler, R. H., Sunstein, C., & Balz, J. P. (2012). Choice architecture. In E. Shafir (Eds.), The behavioral foundations of public policy. (pp. 428-439). Princeton,NJ: Princeton University Press.
[45] Wood, W. (2017). Habit in personality and social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(4), 389-403.
doi: 10.1177/1088868317720362URLpmid: 28737111
[46] Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S., Lin, P.-Y., & Ru?nger, D. (2014). Habits in dual process models. In J. W. Sherman, B. Gawronski & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories of the social mind(pp. 371-385). New York: Guilford.
[47] Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2007). A new look at habits and the habit-goal interface. Psychology Review, 114(4), 843-863.
[48] Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2016). Healthy through habit: Interventions for initiating and maintaining health behavior change. Behavioral Science & Policy, 2(2016), 71-83.
[49] Wood, W., & Ru?nger, D. (2016). Psychology of habit. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 289-314.
URLpmid: 26361052
[50] Yin, H. H., & Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Review Neuroscience, 7(6), 464-476.




[1]李朋波, 孙雨晴, 雷铭. 权力何以授予:目标理论视角下领导授权行为的形成机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(7): 1167-1182.
[2]吕小康, 武迪, 隋晓阳, 汪新建, 程婕婷. 从“理性人”到“行为人”:公共政策研究的行为科学转向[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(12): 2249-2259.
[3]吴玄娜. 程序公正与权威信任:公共政策可接受性机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(8): 1147-1158.
[4]马原啸;冉光明;陈旭. 不安全依恋者注意偏向的形成机制及神经基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(3): 392-401.
[5]赵红丹. 员工强制性组织公民行为的多层次形成机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(8): 1218-1225.
[6]刘腾飞; 徐富明;孙彦;张军伟;蒋多. 基于体验效用的幸福及其测量[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(7): 1087-1095.
[7]张文新;陈光辉;林崇德. 应用发展科学
——一门研究人与社会发展的新兴学科
[J]. 心理科学进展, 2009, 17(2): 251-260.
[8]罗清旭;杨鑫辉. 心理学研究与公共政策的制定[J]. 心理科学进展, 2001, 9(1): 77-81.





PDF全文下载地址:

http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/downloadArticleFile.do?attachType=PDF&id=5114
相关话题/科学 心理 公共政策 奖励 社会